Introduction: Loneliness poses a significant health problem and existing psychological interventions have shown only limited positive effects on loneliness. Based on preliminary evidence for impaired oxytocin signaling in trait-like loneliness, the current proof-of-concept study used a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled design to probe intranasal oxytocin (OT) as an adjunct to a short-term modular-based group intervention for individuals suffering from high trait-like loneliness (HL, UCLA Loneliness Scale ≥55). Methods: Seventy-eight healthy HL adults (56 women) received five weekly group psychotherapy sessions. HL participants received OT or placebo before the intervention sessions. Primary outcomes were trait-like loneliness measured at baseline, after the intervention, and again at two follow-up time points (3 weeks and 3 months), and, assessed at each session, state loneliness (visual analog scale), perceived stress (Perceived Stress Scale, PSS-10), quality of life (World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index, WHO-5), and the therapeutic relationship (Group Questionnaire, GQ-D). Results: The psychological intervention was associated with significantly reduced perceived stress and improved trait-like loneliness across treatment groups, which was still evident at the 3-month follow-up. OT had no significant effect on trait-like loneliness, quality of life, or perceived stress. However, compared to placebo, OT significantly facilitated the decrease in state loneliness within sessions and significantly improved positive bonding between the group members. Conclusion: Despite significantly improved trait-like loneliness after the intervention, OT did not significantly augment this effect. Further studies are needed to determine optimal intervention designs to translate the observed acute effects of OT into long-term benefits.

Loneliness is defined as an aversive emotional reaction resulting from a perceived deficiency in the quality or quantity of social connections [1]. From an evolutionary standpoint, loneliness is postulated to be adaptive since it serves as an aversive signal, which motivates people to reconnect, much like hunger motivates to eat [2, 3]. Persistent loneliness can trigger neurobiological and behavioral mechanisms with detrimental effects on mental and physical health [4‒6]. Despite a close phenotypical overlap with depression and social anxiety, loneliness has been identified as a distinct construct [7, 8]. The prevalence of loneliness varies depending on assessment criteria and demographic factors such as age [9, 10], but loneliness has been increasingly recognized as an important sociopolitical and public health issue [11‒13].

A recent meta-analysis showed that psychological interventions in general are effective in reducing loneliness [14]. Previous interventions included several different approaches, ranging from cognitive behavioral therapies to mindfulness-based interventions, and social skills training. Averaged across interventions, the meta-analysis yielded a small to medium effect size of g = 0.43, favoring the interventions against control conditions. This estimate, however, might be too optimistic as (1) there was no active control group in two thirds of the 31 analyzed studies, (2) only about 30% of the studies had a low risk of bias, and (3) the few larger studies showed smaller effect sizes than most studies with fewer participants. In addition, most studies had a very high attrition rate of up to 45% (mean 10.5%), most likely caused by long interventions (a mean of ten sessions of 1–2 h; duration ranging from 2 weeks to 12 months).

Considering the significant impact of loneliness for individuals and society, there is an urgent need to develop more effective interventions [15]. A potential strategy is the pharmacological augmentation of psychological interventions via the hypothalamic peptide oxytocin (OT). OT is a key modulator of social cognition and behaviors including fear learning [16, 17], stress response [18, 19], parental and romantic bonding [20‒23], empathy and social value representations [24, 25], and interpersonal trust [26‒28]. Importantly, there is preliminary evidence that a chronic lack of social connections can interfere with OT-related social functions. In fact, reduced interpersonal trust has been linked to decreased oxytocinergic activity in response to positive social interactions in individuals with trait-like loneliness [29]. While previous studies yielded heterogenous findings about OT augmentation of psychotherapy [30, 31], a potential effect of OT as an adjunct to a modular-based intervention against loneliness has not been tested yet.

Thus, our randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled proof-of-concept trial investigated intranasal OT as a potential adjunct to a psychological short-term intervention for participants suffering from high trait-like loneliness (HL; UCLA Loneliness Scale ≥55 [32]). The aim of the study was to evaluate the feasibility, acceptability, and first evidence on the effectiveness of an OT augmentation of a modular-based psychotherapy for loneliness. Based on the cognitive behavioral model of loneliness [4, 33], our short-term group intervention consisting of five weekly sessions combined techniques from different evidence-based psychotherapies and targeted critical psychological mechanisms of trait-like loneliness, such as social hopelessness, distrust, limited social skills, and maladaptive thinking (dysfunctional cognitions).

According to the proof-of-concept nature of the study, we chose a broader spectrum of primary and secondary outcomes. Given the meta-analytic findings of psychological interventions on loneliness, the first of our preregistered hypotheses was that the group intervention would reduce trait-like loneliness (H1.1) and state loneliness (H1.2). We further expected that the psychological intervention would reduce perceived stress (H2.1) and increase quality of life (H2.2). Our third hypothesis was that OT would facilitate the development of positive bonding (PB) as perceived by the participants (H3). Moreover, we hypothesized that OT would augment the positive effects of the intervention on trait-like loneliness (H4.1), state loneliness (H4.2), perceived stress (H4.3), and quality of life (H4.4). Finally, we tested the effects of the psychological intervention on secondary outcomes consisting of loneliness-related depressive symptoms and social anxiety. Moreover, we explored OT effects on the therapeutic relationship as seen by the therapist. Further exploratory analyses were conducted to examine possible long-term effects of the intervention with follow-up measurements 3 weeks and 3 months after the intervention. To further characterize the HL participants, we included a control group involving healthy participants with low loneliness (LL) scores that did not receive the intervention.

Study Design

The study design was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT04137432), and the analysis plan was preregistered before conducting any analyses (https://osf.io/qpfw3). Healthy subjects suffering from trait-like loneliness (UCLA Loneliness Scale; UCLA-L scale [32, 34]; i.e., ≥55, i.e., at least 1.5 standard deviations above the mean score in the original validation study, cf. [32]) took part in the randomized, double-blind, parallel-group study design and received a modular-based group intervention to reduce loneliness. In half of the intervention groups (HL individuals), participants received 24 international units (IUs) of OT 30 min before the group sessions, and the other half received a placebo spray (PLC), which contained identical ingredients except for the peptide itself. There is strong evidence that intranasal OT reaches the brain [35], and 24 IU have been identified as the most effective dose to modulate amygdala reactivity in men [36]. Groups were randomized to each treatment condition with all participants of a group receiving the same treatment for all intervention sessions, i.e., either OT or PLC. The allocation sequence was concealed from the therapists and the participants throughout the whole trial. As a control group, 49 healthy participants with low loneliness scores (UCLA-L scale ≤35) were included. The LL participants did not receive an intervention or treatment but were tested with the same psychometric measures as the HL participants at study entry and after a time interval comparable to the duration of the intervention of HL participants (see Experimental procedure). All participants (HL and LL) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging before the start and after the end of the intervention (results will be presented elsewhere).

Participants

We recruited participants via online advertisements and postings on social media platforms, a regional newspaper article, and physical flyers in healthcare as well as university facilities from June 2019 to November 2021. Interested individuals were asked to fill out an online questionnaire that included the UCLA-L scale and questions regarding the exclusion criteria (current psychiatric illness, current psychiatric medication or psychotherapy, MRI contraindication such as claustrophobia, and OT contraindication such as cardiovascular diseases or pregnancy). A total of 167 HL individuals who fulfilled our inclusion criteria (UCLA-L score ≥55, aged 18–65 years) were invited to a first screening session (T0) which included an in-depth query of the exclusion criteria. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) [37] was conducted to rule out participants fulfilling the criteria for a psychiatric disorder. Included participants were then allocated to the next possible intervention group after finishing the first fMRI session (T1). All participants who were assigned to an intervention group after T1 and who participated at the first intervention session (T2) were included with respect to the intention-to-treat sample. In total, we enrolled 79 HL participants receiving either OT or PLC, resulting in 12 intervention groups. We excluded one participant from analyses because of concurrent participation in psychotherapy. Thus, the final sample consisted of 43 HL participants in the OT group and 35 HL participants in the PLC group. This sample size is in line with our a-priori power analysis (cf. online suppl. information; for all online suppl. material, see https://doi.org/10.1159/000538752). The 49 LL participants had to fulfill the same inclusion and exclusion criteria as the HL participants, except for the UCLA-L score, which had to be under 35. For details of the enrollment process, see online supplementary Figure S1.

Experimental Procedure

All HL participants meeting our inclusion criteria evaluated through the online questionnaire were invited to a screening session (T0; see section Participants), during which psychiatric symptoms and social network characteristics were assessed. Following the screening session and before the start of the respective group intervention, participants completed further questionnaires during the first testing session (T1) to assess baseline trait-like loneliness and quality of life. After finishing T1, participants were assigned to an intervention group with randomized treatment conditions (i.e., randomization took place after T1). The group intervention then started with a first introduction session (T2) and was carried out by one of two trained psychotherapists. The intervention was limited to five weekly sessions (including the first introduction session; T2).

Given the kinetics of central OT effects [36], the duration of each intervention session was limited to 1 h. Notably, no OT was administered during T2 as previous findings indicated possible anxiogenic effects of OT in therapeutic situations involving strangers [31]. Therefore, the first group session allowed the participants to become familiar with the therapeutic context prior to the first OT administration. Due to recruiting difficulties during the Corona pandemic, we had to adapt our original study protocol such that the minimum group size was reduced to four individuals.

Our short-term group intervention included modules focusing on the critical psychological phenomena of long-lasting loneliness, such as social hopelessness, distrust, limited social skills, and dysfunctional cognition, leading to a lack of rewarding interpersonal relationships. As loneliness is not a psychiatric disorder, most psychotherapies have not been directed toward this problem specifically, and yet many psychotherapeutic techniques are well suited to address the behavioral, interpersonal, cognitive, and emotional correlates of loneliness. Utilizing techniques from different evidence-based psychotherapies matching the psychological correlates of loneliness, we developed a modular approach using tools mainly from interpersonal psychotherapy, acceptance and commitment therapy, cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy, and social skill training. For details of the content of the psychological intervention, see the online supplementary information. After completing the intervention, participants filled out questionnaires regarding their loneliness, quality of life, and psychiatric symptomatology on a separate testing day (T7). Follow-up measurements of the aforementioned variables were further collected 3 weeks (T8) and 3 months (T9) after finishing the intervention. Participants of the LL group completed testing sessions T0, T1, and T7 in a comparable time interval without any treatment in between. A schematic overview of the study design for HL participants is shown in Figure 1.

Fig. 1.

Illustration of the study design. The top row shows the time frame and collected measurements during the study. The second row depicts the five intervention sessions with an overview of the topics of each session. ACT, acceptance and commitment therapy; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CBASP, cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy; CBT, cognitive behavioral therapy; CTQ, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; GQ-D, Group Questionnaire; HAQ, Help Alliance Questionnaire; IPT, interpersonal psychotherapy; LSAS, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; PSS-10, Perceived Stress Scale; SN, Social Network Index; T0–T9, testing session 0 (screening)–9; UCLA-L, UCLA Loneliness Scale; WHO-5, World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index.

Fig. 1.

Illustration of the study design. The top row shows the time frame and collected measurements during the study. The second row depicts the five intervention sessions with an overview of the topics of each session. ACT, acceptance and commitment therapy; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CBASP, cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy; CBT, cognitive behavioral therapy; CTQ, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; GQ-D, Group Questionnaire; HAQ, Help Alliance Questionnaire; IPT, interpersonal psychotherapy; LSAS, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; PSS-10, Perceived Stress Scale; SN, Social Network Index; T0–T9, testing session 0 (screening)–9; UCLA-L, UCLA Loneliness Scale; WHO-5, World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index.

Close modal

Measures

We measured trait-like loneliness using a validated German version of the revised UCLA-L scale with a 20-item 4-point Likert scale with scores ranging from 20 to 80 [32]. Specifically, the UCLA-L scale was used in an online questionnaire to identify participants who fulfilled our inclusion criteria (UCLA-L score of ≥55). Moreover, we assessed the UCLA-L scale at T1 (before the start of the intervention) and T7 (after the intervention) and at both follow-up time points 3 weeks (T8) and 3 months (T9) after the end of the intervention period. Furthermore, to assess state loneliness, participants rated their current feelings of loneliness on a visual analog scale (VAS) from 0 (“not lonely at all”) to 100 (“very lonely”) from sessions T2–T6 twice, i.e., at the beginning and at the end of each session.

Quality of life was assessed by 5 items of the World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index with a 5-point Likert scale with scores ranging from 0 to 25 (WHO-5 [38]), and subjective stress was measured by 10 items of the Perceived Stress Scale with a 5-point Likert scale with scores ranging from 0 to 40 (PSS-10 [39, 40]). Participants completed both questionnaires at the beginning of each testing session (T1–T7) and at the follow-up measurements (T8 and T9). As participants were tested once a week during the group intervention period, instructions of the PSS-10 were changed for these measurements and referred to perceived stress in the last week instead of the last month (as assessed in the remaining testing sessions). To assess the therapeutic relationship toward the whole group, other individual participants, and the therapists, the three subscales of the Group Questionnaire GQ-D (30 items with a 7-point Likert scale), PB (range 4–28 for other participants and the therapist; range 5–35 for the whole group), positive working relationship (range 4–28 for the therapist and other participants), and negative relationships (range 3–21 for the therapist, other participants, and the whole group) [41, 42] were completed by the group members at the end of each intervention session (T2–T6). The perceived therapeutic alliance by the therapist was assessed at the end of each intervention session (T2–T6) for exploratory purposes using the Help Alliance Questionnaire (12 item with a 6-point Likert scale) with scorers ranging from 12 to 72 (HAQ [43]).

As secondary outcomes, depressive and social anxiety symptoms were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (21 items with a 4-point Likert scale) with scores ranging from 0 to 63 (BDI, [44]) and the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (50 items with a 4-point Likert scale) with scores ranging from 0 to 72 (LSAS, [45]) at the screening session (T0), after finishing the intervention period (T7), and at both follow-up measurements (T8 and T9). For exploratory purposes and to characterize the sample in a more comprehensive way, we also assessed the social network size and diversity (i.e., number of social roles, number of people in one’s social network, and embedded networks) by using the social network index (SN [46]). Like depressive symptomatology and social anxiety, the SN was completed at T0, T7, and at both follow-up measurements (T8 and T9). At the screening session (T0), the history of childhood maltreatment was assessed by the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; 25 items with a 5-point Liker scale with scores ranging from 25 to 125 [47]). For additional exploratory analyses of affective states, see online supplementary information and supplementary Figure S1.

Statistical Analysis

We performed intention-to-treat analyses to avoid inflated type I errors due to the exclusion of participants who did not complete the intervention. Intention-to-treat analyses provide a more real-world perspective on the efficacy, tolerability, and acceptability of an intervention. Therefore, all participants who received at least the first intervention session (T2) were considered for statistical analyses. To account for the multilevel, longitudinal data structure, we analyzed primary outcomes with linear mixed models with repeated measures. The p values of post hoc comparisons were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni-Holm method.

To test our hypotheses, the following fixed-effects factors were included as predictors in their respective models:

  • 1.

    To test the assumption that the psychological intervention has positive effects on feelings of loneliness (H1.1), testing session (T1, T7) was fitted as a fixed-effects factor to predict UCLA-L scores at both measurement occasions. For the model including state loneliness as the dependent variable (H1.2), both within-session time (start/end of intervention session) and testing session (T2, T3, T4, T5, T6) as well as their interaction were fitted as categorical fixed-effects factors. Similarly, both PSS-10 and WHO-5 scores were considered as dependent variables (H2.1 and H2.2). In these models, testing session (T1, T2, T3, T4, T5, T6, T7) was included as categorical fixed-effects factor.

  • 2.

    With respect to OT effects on the therapeutic relationship as experienced by the participants (H3.1 and H3.2), the scores of the GQ-D subscales were the dependent variables, while treatment (PLC, OT), testing session (T2, T3, T4, T5, T6), and the interaction of treatment with testing session were included in the models as categorical fixed-effects factors.

  • 3.

    Likewise, treatment (PLC, OT) and its potential interactions with the respective fixed-effects factors as specified above were included as additional fixed-effects factors to investigate OT effects on loneliness, stress, and quality of life (H4.1–H4.4).

  • 4.

    For all analyses, sex and age (grand mean centered) assessed at T0 were included as covariates. To account for possible influences of the COVID-19 pandemic, an additional binary covariate was included in the models, which specified whether testing sessions were completed prior to the pandemic (i.e., in 2019 or the beginning of 2020) or during the pandemic.

  • 5.

    To explore intervention and OT effects on the secondary outcomes, the same models as specified for the first hypothesis (H1.1 and H4.1, respectively) were used to predict BDI and LSAS scores, respectively.

For further details of the statistical analyses and for further exploratory analyses of sex-dependent OT effects, see the online supplementary information.

Study Sample

HL individuals were characterized by increased psychopathological symptoms compared to the LL control group at study entry (for a comparison of sociodemographic data, see online suppl. Table S1). As such, lonely individuals reported increased depressive symptoms (t(108.4) = 10.24, p < 0.001, d = 1.57), social anxiety (t(117.7) = 8.78, p < 0.001, d = 1.38), and perceived social stress (t(100.9) = 10.88, p < 0.001, d = 1.98) in addition to decreased quality of life (t(119.8) = −8.73, p < 0.001, d = −1.51; see online suppl. Table S2). Moreover, as expected, HL participants reported more severe childhood maltreatment (t(124.8) = 6.55, p < 0.001, d = 1.07) and increased objective social isolation in form of smaller social networks (number of social roles: t(94.6), = −7.63, p < 0.001, d = −1.42; number of people: t(74.6) = −8.31, p < 0.001, d = −1.66; number of embedded networks: t(74) = −7.89, p < 0.001, d = −1.59). For a more detailed description of sociodemographic and psychological variables within the HL group separated by OT and PLC treatment, see Table 1 and online suppl. Tables S3–S4. Across both groups, 96% of all participants completed the intervention, and none reported severe side effects. Neither the attendance rate nor the side effects differed significantly between treatment groups (see online suppl. information and Table S5).

Table 1.

Changes in psychiatric outcomes and social network characteristics in the high loneliness group

Pretreatment (T0/T1)Posttreatment (T7)3-week follow-up (T8)3-month follow-up (T9)
PLC (n = 34)OT (n = 39)PLC (n = 32)OT (n = 37)PLC (n = 32)OT (n = 35)PLC (n = 26)OT (n = 24)FsessionpFtreat-ment*sessionp value
UCLA-L 51.6 (9.0) 55.3 (8.4) 47.8 (9.6) 54.5 (9.2) 50.8 (11.0) 54.3 (10.7) 48.8 (12.7) 52.4 (10.4) 4.03 0.008 1.60 0.19 
WHO-5 12.6 (4.8) 10.5 (4.4) 12.7 (5.2) 10.2 (4.2) 12.4 (5.5) 10.9 (5.1) 11.6 (6.2) 10.7 (5.6) 0.88 0.53 0.45 0.89 
PSS-10 18.7 (5.7) 20.7 (6.1) 17.4 (6.9) 19.9 (6.7) 18.0 (6.9) 18.6 (7.0) 19.3 (5.4) 20.0 (7.6) 1.95 0.05 1.08 0.38 
BDI 12.9 (7.6) 13.4 (9.0) 12.4 (8.0) 15.2 (10.6) 13.2 (10.3) 13.1 (10.0) 13.2 (11.5) 13.8 (9.6) 0.16 0.92 0.69 0.56 
LSAS total 38.6 (24.2) 40.6 (24.9) 44.6 (26.3) 37.7 (26.4) 34.0 (22.5) 32.5 (24.0) 39.1 (29.6) 26.4 (21.8) 4.03 0.008 1.57 0.20 
 LSAS anxiety 21.8 (11.3) 20.7 (11.7) 25.5 (14.3) 21.2 (13.4) 20.8 (11.5) 18.9 (12.1) 22.2 (14.3) 15.7 (11.5) 2.73 0.045 0.70 0.56 
 LSAS avoid. 16.8 (13.9) 19.8 (15.1) 19.2 (15.0) 16.5 (14.7) 13.2 (13.4) 13.5 (13.2) 16.9 (17.1) 10.7 (12.0) 4.84 0.003 2.14 0.10 
SN social roles 4.2 (1.6) 4.1 (1.6) 5.0 (1.7) 4.4 (1.8) 4.4 (1.6) 4.5 (1.9) 4.4 (1.7) 4.3 (1.9) 2.79 0.04 1.35 0.26 
SN people 10.9 (5.3) 10.8 (6.3) 12.8 (6.2) 11.9 (7.1) 10.3 (5.8) 11.4 (6.8) 11.0 (5.5) 9.7 (4.7) 2.70 0.047 1.00 0.39 
SN embed. networks 1.1 (0.8) 1.2 (1.0) 1.5 (1.2) 1.3 (1.0) 1.2 (1.0) 1.2 (1.0) 1.2 (1.1) 1.1 (0.9) 1.58 0.19 0.54 0.66 
Pretreatment (T0/T1)Posttreatment (T7)3-week follow-up (T8)3-month follow-up (T9)
PLC (n = 34)OT (n = 39)PLC (n = 32)OT (n = 37)PLC (n = 32)OT (n = 35)PLC (n = 26)OT (n = 24)FsessionpFtreat-ment*sessionp value
UCLA-L 51.6 (9.0) 55.3 (8.4) 47.8 (9.6) 54.5 (9.2) 50.8 (11.0) 54.3 (10.7) 48.8 (12.7) 52.4 (10.4) 4.03 0.008 1.60 0.19 
WHO-5 12.6 (4.8) 10.5 (4.4) 12.7 (5.2) 10.2 (4.2) 12.4 (5.5) 10.9 (5.1) 11.6 (6.2) 10.7 (5.6) 0.88 0.53 0.45 0.89 
PSS-10 18.7 (5.7) 20.7 (6.1) 17.4 (6.9) 19.9 (6.7) 18.0 (6.9) 18.6 (7.0) 19.3 (5.4) 20.0 (7.6) 1.95 0.05 1.08 0.38 
BDI 12.9 (7.6) 13.4 (9.0) 12.4 (8.0) 15.2 (10.6) 13.2 (10.3) 13.1 (10.0) 13.2 (11.5) 13.8 (9.6) 0.16 0.92 0.69 0.56 
LSAS total 38.6 (24.2) 40.6 (24.9) 44.6 (26.3) 37.7 (26.4) 34.0 (22.5) 32.5 (24.0) 39.1 (29.6) 26.4 (21.8) 4.03 0.008 1.57 0.20 
 LSAS anxiety 21.8 (11.3) 20.7 (11.7) 25.5 (14.3) 21.2 (13.4) 20.8 (11.5) 18.9 (12.1) 22.2 (14.3) 15.7 (11.5) 2.73 0.045 0.70 0.56 
 LSAS avoid. 16.8 (13.9) 19.8 (15.1) 19.2 (15.0) 16.5 (14.7) 13.2 (13.4) 13.5 (13.2) 16.9 (17.1) 10.7 (12.0) 4.84 0.003 2.14 0.10 
SN social roles 4.2 (1.6) 4.1 (1.6) 5.0 (1.7) 4.4 (1.8) 4.4 (1.6) 4.5 (1.9) 4.4 (1.7) 4.3 (1.9) 2.79 0.04 1.35 0.26 
SN people 10.9 (5.3) 10.8 (6.3) 12.8 (6.2) 11.9 (7.1) 10.3 (5.8) 11.4 (6.8) 11.0 (5.5) 9.7 (4.7) 2.70 0.047 1.00 0.39 
SN embed. networks 1.1 (0.8) 1.2 (1.0) 1.5 (1.2) 1.3 (1.0) 1.2 (1.0) 1.2 (1.0) 1.2 (1.1) 1.1 (0.9) 1.58 0.19 0.54 0.66 

Values are mean and standard deviation.

BDI, Beck Depression Inventory, Version II; LSAS, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; LSAS anxiety, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale – anxiety subscale; LSAS avoid., Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale – avoidance subscale; OT, intranasal oxytocin treatment; PLC, placebo treatment; PSS-10, Perceived Stress Scale; SN, Social Network Index; SN embed. networks, Social Network Index – embedded networks; UCLA-L, UCLA Loneliness Scale; WHO-5, World Health Organization Five Well-Being Index.

Intervention Effects

As expected, across all HL participants, the psychological intervention was associated with a reduction of trait-like loneliness as evident in significantly decreased UCLA-L scores from study entry at T1 to post intervention at T7 (H1.1) (b = −1.90, SE = 0.87, t(68) = −2.19, p = 0.03, d = −0.33; shown in Fig. 2a). Moreover, for state loneliness, we found a significant interaction of testing sessions and within-session time (H1.2) (F(4, 315.2) = 2.60, p = 0.04). Thus, changes in state feelings of loneliness from the start to the end of an intervention session significantly differed between testing sessions (T2–T6). Specifically, post hoc tests revealed an initial increase in state loneliness at T3, which differed significantly from the decrease observed in the following testing sessions (difference from pre to post at session T3 vs. T4: b = 11.24, SE = 4.14, t(316) = 2.72, p = 0.06, d = 0.31; T3 versus T6: b = 11.60, SE = 4.12, t(313) = 2.82, p = 0.05, d = 0.29; all other comparisons of changes in pre-post values between testing sessions t < 2.00, p > 0.37).

Fig. 2.

a The group-based intervention was associated with a significant reduction of trait-like loneliness in individuals suffering from high loneliness (HL) from study entry at T1 to the post-measurement at T7. b Likewise, the group-based intervention was linked to a significant decrease in perceived stress across sessions (T1–T7). Intranasal oxytocin (OT) had no significant effect on trait-like loneliness or perceived stress. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. OT, oxytocin; PSS-10, Perceived Stress Scale; PLC, placebo; T1–T7, testing session 1–7; UCLA-L, UCLA Loneliness Scale. *p < 0.05.

Fig. 2.

a The group-based intervention was associated with a significant reduction of trait-like loneliness in individuals suffering from high loneliness (HL) from study entry at T1 to the post-measurement at T7. b Likewise, the group-based intervention was linked to a significant decrease in perceived stress across sessions (T1–T7). Intranasal oxytocin (OT) had no significant effect on trait-like loneliness or perceived stress. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. OT, oxytocin; PSS-10, Perceived Stress Scale; PLC, placebo; T1–T7, testing session 1–7; UCLA-L, UCLA Loneliness Scale. *p < 0.05.

Close modal

In addition to the observed reduction of trait-like loneliness and in line with hypothesis 2.1, perceived psychosocial stress decreased over time (main effect of testing session: F(6, 373.5) = 2.62, p = 0.02, shown in Fig. 2b and online suppl. information). However, no significant intervention effects were observed for quality of life (H2.2.; main effect session: F(6,387) = 0.69, p = 0.65), depressive symptomatology (b = 0.72, SE = 0.95, t(70.3) = 0.76, p = 0.45, d = 0.14), or social anxiety (b = 1.63, SE = 2.09, t(69.8) = 0.78, p = 0.44, d = 0.06).

OT Effects

In line with our hypotheses, the OT treatment had a significant positive effect on the therapeutic relationship of HL participants (H3). Importantly, while a positive working relationship significantly improved from the start of the psychological intervention at T2 to the end at T6 across all participants (main effect of testing session: F(4,202) = 14.35, p < 0.001), OT specifically boosted PB. In particular, PB to the whole group increased significantly more in intervention groups receiving OT (interaction of treatment and testing session: F(4,234.5) = 2.83, p = 0.03; shown in Fig. 3). At the end of the intervention (T6), PB to the whole group was significantly increased in groups with OT treatment, but not with PLC. This effect remained significant in an additional model with the start value (T2) used as a covariate (cf. online suppl. information). No further significant effects of treatment or testing session were observed for any of the GQ-D subscales (further main effects of treatment or testing session and interactions of treatment and testing session: all F <2.28, all p > 0.06).

Fig. 3.

PB to the whole group increased significantly stronger in groups receiving intranasal oxytocin (T2–T6) compared to placebo. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. GQ-D, Group Questionnaire; OT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo; T2–T6, testing session 2–6. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Fig. 3.

PB to the whole group increased significantly stronger in groups receiving intranasal oxytocin (T2–T6) compared to placebo. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. GQ-D, Group Questionnaire; OT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo; T2–T6, testing session 2–6. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Close modal

We did not observe a significant main or interaction effect of OT on trait-like loneliness (H4.1; all F <4.42, all p > 0.06; mean decrease ±SD in the PLC group: −3.63 ± 6.57, OT: −0.43 ± 7.49). Importantly, however, OT significantly improved the psychological intervention effects on state loneliness (H4.2) (interaction of treatment with within-session time: F(1,306.3) = 4.45, p = 0.04): Across intervention sessions, OT was associated with a decrease in state loneliness from the start of the testing session to the end (b = −4.62, SE = 1.78, t(305.6) = −2.60, p = 0.04, d = −0.34), whereas state loneliness did not change significantly within a testing session after PLC treatment (b = 1.02, SE = 2.00, t(306.8) = 0.51, p > 0.99, d = −0.10; shown in Fig. 4). Notably, further exploratory post hoc tests indicated that OT prevented the observed increase in state loneliness at session T3 (see Intervention Effects), which was evident after PLC treatment (b = 14.16, SE = 4.30, t(309) = 3.29, p = 0.02, d = 0.48) but absent after OT treatment (b = −0.99, SE = 3.83, t(304) = −0.26, p > 0.99, d = −0.03). No further significant effects of OT were observed for our primary or secondary outcomes (main effects or interactions of treatment; all F <4.42, all p > 0.06; see also online suppl. Table S4).

Fig. 4.

a State loneliness was significantly lower after the intervention sessions compared to the start of the sessions in intervention groups with intranasal oxytocin treatment, but not with placebo. b In the placebo groups, state loneliness significantly increased at session T3 and this effect was absent in the oxytocin groups. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. OT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo; T2–T6, testing session 2–6. *p < 0.05.

Fig. 4.

a State loneliness was significantly lower after the intervention sessions compared to the start of the sessions in intervention groups with intranasal oxytocin treatment, but not with placebo. b In the placebo groups, state loneliness significantly increased at session T3 and this effect was absent in the oxytocin groups. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. OT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo; T2–T6, testing session 2–6. *p < 0.05.

Close modal

Exploratory Outcomes

In a first step, we explored whether positive effects of the OT treatment on the therapeutic relationship were also evident in an improved relationship as experienced by the therapists. In fact, the therapeutic relationship measured by the HAQ showed a descriptively overall better result in OT groups compared to the PLC groups (mean ± SD for OT: 46.70 ± 3.48; PLC: 44.27 ± 3.23). This pattern of results was observed for both HAQ subscales (relationship to the patients subscale for OT: 27.10 ± 2.08; PLC: 26.12 ± 1.93; satisfaction with the therapeutic outcome subscale for OT: 19.60 ± 1.61; PLC: 18.15 ± 1.56).

We then explored whether positive intervention effects on UCLA-L scores and perceived psychosocial stress were still evident in our follow-up measurements at 3 weeks (T8) and 3 months (T9) after completing the intervention. Importantly, across all HL participants, trait-like loneliness scores at T8 and T9 did not differ from those collected after finishing the intervention at T7 (p = 0.77), indicating a long-lasting decrease in loneliness. Along these lines, loneliness at T9 was still significantly decreased compared to study entry at T1 (b = −3.10, SE = 0.98, t(186) = 3.15, p = 0.01, d = −0.41; all other post hoc comparisons: t < 2.14, p > 0.13). In contrast, no significant long-term effects were observed for perceived psychosocial stress (all post hoc comparisons: t < 2.13, p > 0.16). However, a significant main effect of testing session after including T8 and T9 in the models indicated a delayed intervention effect on social anxiety (F(3,188.7) = 4.22, p = 0.006). Post hoc tests revealed a significant reduction of social anxiety from study entry at T0 as well as from T7 after finishing the intervention to the first follow-up measurement at T8 (T8 vs. T0: b = −6.28, SE = 2.46, t(189) = −2.55, p = 0.046, d = −0.38; T8 versus T7: b = −7.82, SE = 2.48, t(187) = −3.15, p = 0.01, d = −0.45; all other post hoc comparisons: t < 2.20, p > 0.08). No further significant delayed effects of the intervention were observed for stress, quality of life, or depressive symptomatology (all main effects of testing session: F <0.90, p > 0.52). Likewise, OT treatment had no significant effects on our primary or secondary outcomes at the follow-up measurements at T8 and T9 (all interactions of treatment with testing session after including T8 and T9 in the models: F <2.14, p > 0.09; see also Table 1).

Finally, we explored whether the psychological intervention had positive effects on objective social isolation (i.e., social network sizes) in addition to the reported effects on loneliness. Indeed, the number of social roles of the participants and the number of people within one’s network increased from study entry to T7 (number of roles: b = 0.45, SE = 0.17, t(70.3) = 2.61, p = 0.01, d = 0.37; number of people: b = 1.20, SE = 0.61, t(70.2) = 1.96, p = 0.05, d = 0.31), whereas the number of embedded networks within one’s social network did not significantly change (b = 0.18, SE = 0.12, t(70.6) = 1.50, p = 0.14, d = 0.24). Changes within the social networks were independent of OT (all main effects or interactions with treatment: F <1.42, p > 0.23; see Table 1 and online suppl. Tables S2).

This proof-of-concept study aimed to examine the feasibility, acceptability, and first evidence on the effectiveness of OT as an adjunct to a short-term psychological group intervention against loneliness. None of the participants experienced severe side effects and we observed a high attendance and low dropout rate, and these were not significantly affected by the treatment. The psychological group intervention was associated with a small but significant improvement of trait-like loneliness and psychosocial stress, and the former effect was still evident 3 months after the end of the intervention. Contrary to our hypothesis, OT had no further augmenting effect on these outcomes. Consistent with our hypothesis, however, OT significantly decreased state loneliness scores from the start to the finish of each group session, an effect not seen in PLC groups. Also consistent with our hypothesis, OT significantly improved PB to the whole group. Furthermore, neither the intervention nor the OT treatment significantly changed quality of life or depressive symptoms. Interestingly, however, exploratory analyses revealed a delayed intervention effect of reduced social anxiety 3 weeks and 3 months after the group sessions.

Intriguingly, OT significantly facilitated the decrease in state loneliness within the sessions, but the peptide produced no significant effect on trait-like loneliness. More specifically, the deterioration of state loneliness during T3 in the PLC groups was prevented by OT. This second group session included psychoeducation regarding the negative consequences and the vicious circle of development and maintenance of trait-like loneliness. In the following sessions, there was a consistent within-session improvement of state loneliness. These following sessions focused more on symptom management and skill learning, possibly more likely to engender hope and relief. It is well known from different process-monitored psychotherapies that an initial problem activation may cause a temporary exacerbation of symptoms [48, 49]. These results provide further support for the notion that social effects of OT vary substantially between contexts [25, 50, 51]. Because of the observation that the adjunct application of 40 IU of OT before the first psychotherapy session resulted in acute anxiogenesis in patients with depression [31], we implemented a first meet-and-greet session without OT to create a safe environment. Nevertheless, OT did not significantly influence trait-like loneliness. Of note, trait-like loneliness was assessed with the UCLA loneliness questionnaire that does not directly ask about loneliness. By contrast, the VAS explicitly measures state loneliness. These differences in the assessment method may have contributed to the observed differential OT effect. However, because significant effects of OT on PB were evident in later sessions, it is also conceivable that more sessions are needed for positive group dynamics to impact the therapeutic group process itself and, thereby, trait-like loneliness. Likewise, we did not observe a significant intervention effect on subjective well-being, although there is a close link between loneliness and quality of life [52]. Along these lines, a longer intervention or a longer follow-up period may be necessary for changes to become evident. The intervention had no significant effect on social anxiety in the days following the intervention (at T7), but both the fear and avoidance of social situations were significantly reduced 3 weeks (T8) and 3 months (T9) after the intervention. Participants may need more time to learn and to practice new skills and behaviors in daily life and thereby reduce avoidance and anxiety through new experiences (confrontation) and cognitive change, both in the PLC and OT group. In line with this idea, a delayed reduction of anxiety has also been observed for group psychotherapy for major depressive disorder [53].

The OT-induced increase in PB with the whole group is consistent with previous studies showing a significant OT effect on interpersonal trust [26, 28, 54], at least in participants with a low disposition to trust [27] like trait-like lonely individuals [29]. Mechanistically, OT may have influenced the biobehavioral synchrony between participants given previous findings that trait-like loneliness is associated with reduced social synchrony [55] and that OT can improve synchrony in group psychotherapies [56]. OT-based synchrony predicted treatment outcomes in a dyadic psychotherapy for depression [57], but an increase in OT levels during psychodynamic treatment sessions correlated with more instances of conflict and rupture in the alliance with the therapist [58]. We did not measure OT levels during the sessions in the present study, but improved bonding after exogenously elevated OT levels suggest that OT effects differ between cognitive behavioral and psychodynamic interventions or might reflect differences between patients with depression and trait-like lonely individuals. Furthermore, the fact that the therapists also rated the therapeutic relationship more positively in OT groups than PLC groups indicates that the OT-related bonding effects could be perceived by the therapists.

The findings of the present study need to be considered in the context of the following limitations: The intervention consisted of five sessions that each were limited to 1 h to account for the pharmacodynamic effects of OT [36]. Considering the small effect size associated with the short-term intervention in this study, more pronounced reductions of loneliness may require longer programs. However, the advantage of increased effectiveness must be balanced with a potentially reduced acceptance in the form of higher dropout rates. Clearly, the effectiveness of interventions may depend on the severity and chronicity of loneliness and comorbidities. Although we screened for psychiatric disorders with clinical interviews, the HL participants reported significantly more depression and social anxiety symptoms than the LL control group. In light of the observation that loneliness predicted subsequent changes in depressive symptomatology, but not vice versa [7], loneliness interventions may be more effective when administered before the manifestation of other clinical symptoms. The lack of a sham intervention or waiting group hampers any conclusions about the efficacy of the group-based psychological intervention. In addition, the preliminary findings of this study need to be replicated with a larger sample and broader exclusion criteria. Given the proof-of-concept design, no participants with mental illness were included, and it is unclear whether the observed findings apply to participants with greater functional impairments. The significant, but relatively small decrease in trait-like loneliness after the psychological intervention is not surprising considering the short-term design in the current study. Furthermore, the psychological intervention only transiently increased social network size and diversity (as expected due to the nature of the group intervention), while the reduction of trait-like loneliness persisted even 3 months later. As such, the psychological intervention effect on loneliness cannot be explained as a byproduct of altered objective social isolation. These findings support the notion that social isolation and trait-like loneliness are two independent constructs and that one may occur without the other [5]. Future studies with ecological momentary assessments are warranted to probe how intervention-related behavioral changes are implemented in real life.

Collectively, the results of this proof-of-concept study show that OT as an adjunct to a short-term group-based psychological intervention is feasible, acceptable, and has a positive effect in reducing state loneliness and in enhancing positive group relationships. However, we did not observe a significant OT effect on the reduction of trait loneliness. As loneliness is one of the strongest social determinants of somatic and mental health [13], further research is warranted to determine how significant augmentation effects of OT on acute loneliness can be intensified, translated, and carried forward to long-term benefits to improve trait-like loneliness.

A preprint of this article is published at the medRxiv preprint server (https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.10.30.23297746).

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Medical Faculty of the University of Bonn (74/14) and conducted in accordance with the latest revision of the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants provided written informed consent after receiving a complete description of the study.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

S.G.S.-T., R.H., and D.S. were supported by a German-Israel Foundation for Scientific Research and Development grant (GIF, I-1428-105.4/2017). R.H. and D.S. were supported by German Research Foundation (DFG) grants (HU 1302/11-1 and SCHE 1913/5-1 and HU 1302/18-1 and SCHE 1913/7-1).

R.B., J.L., and D.S. designed the experiment; R.B., M.B., J.G., and J.L. conducted the experiments; R.B., J.L., M.B., and D.S. analyzed the data. R.B., R.H., S.S.-T., A.K., M.B., J.G., M.L., E.S., J.S., A.P., J.L., and D.S. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript in its current version.

Additional Information

Jana Lieberz and Dirk Scheele contributed equally to this work (shared senior authorship).

The data that support the findings of the present study are openly available in the repository of the Open Science Foundation at https://osf.io/kdy4p/.

1.
Peplau
LA
,
Caldwell
MA
.
Loneliness: a cognitive analysis
.
Essence
.
1978
;
2
:
207
20
.
2.
Cacioppo
JT
,
Cacioppo
S
.
Social relationships and health: the toxic effects of perceived social isolation
.
Soc Personal Psychol Compass
.
2014
;
8
(
2
):
58
72
.
3.
Tomova
L
,
Wang
KL
,
Thompson
T
,
Matthews
GA
,
Takahashi
A
,
Tye
KM
, et al
.
Acute social isolation evokes midbrain craving responses similar to hunger
.
Nat Neurosci
.
2020
;
23
(
12
):
1597
605
.
4.
Cacioppo
JT
,
Hawkley
LC
.
Perceived social isolation and cognition
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2009
;
13
(
10
):
447
54
.
5.
Holt-Lunstad
J
,
Smith
TB
,
Baker
M
,
Harris
T
,
Stephenson
D
.
Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review
.
Perspect Psychol Sci
.
2015
;
10
(
2
):
227
37
.
6.
Quadt
L
,
Esposito
G
,
Critchley
HD
,
Garfinkel
SN
.
Brain-body interactions underlying the association of loneliness with mental and physical health
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2020
;
116
:
283
300
.
7.
Cacioppo
JT
,
Hawkley
LC
,
Thisted
RA
.
Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study
.
Psychol Aging
.
2010
;
25
(
2
):
453
63
.
8.
Lieberz
J
,
Shamay-Tsoory
SG
,
Saporta
N
,
Kanterman
A
,
Gorni
J
,
Esser
T
, et al
.
Behavioral and neural dissociation of social anxiety and loneliness
.
J Neurosci
.
2022
;
42
(
12
):
2570
83
.
9.
Mund
M
,
Freuding
MM
,
Möbius
K
,
Horn
N
,
Neyer
FJ
.
The stability and change of loneliness across the life span: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies
.
Pers Soc Psychol Rev
.
2020
;
24
(
1
):
24
52
.
10.
Luhmann
M
,
Buecker
S
,
Rüsberg
M
.
Loneliness across time and space
.
Nat Rev Psyc
.
2023
;
2
(
1
):
9
23
.
11.
Jeste
DV
,
Lee
EE
,
Cacioppo
S
.
Battling the modern behavioral epidemic of loneliness: suggestions for research and interventions
.
JAMA Psychiatry
.
2020
;
77
(
6
):
553
4
.
12.
Mihalopoulos
C
,
Le
LK-D
,
Chatterton
ML
,
Bucholc
J
,
Holt-Lunstad
J
,
Lim
MH
, et al
.
The economic costs of loneliness: a review of cost-of-illness and economic evaluation studies
.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol
.
2020
;
55
(
7
):
823
36
.
13.
Holt-Lunstad
J
,
Perissinotto
C
.
Social isolation and loneliness as medical issues
.
N Engl J Med
.
2023
;
388
(
3
):
193
5
.
14.
Hickin
N
,
Käll
A
,
Shafran
R
,
Sutcliffe
S
,
Manzotti
G
,
Langan
D
.
The effectiveness of psychological interventions for loneliness: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Clin Psychol Rev
.
2021
;
88
:
102066
.
15.
Morr
M
,
Liu
X
,
Hurlemann
R
,
Becker
B
,
Scheele
D
.
Chronic loneliness: neurocognitive mechanisms and interventions
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2022
;
91
(
4
):
227
37
.
16.
Knobloch
HS
,
Charlet
A
,
Hoffmann
LC
,
Eliava
M
,
Khrulev
S
,
Cetin
AH
, et al
.
Evoked axonal oxytocin release in the central amygdala attenuates fear response
.
Neuron
.
2012
;
73
(
3
):
553
66
.
17.
Eckstein
M
,
Scheele
D
,
Patin
A
,
Preckel
K
,
Becker
B
,
Walter
A
, et al
.
Oxytocin facilitates Pavlovian fear learning in males
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
.
2016
;
41
(
4
):
932
9
.
18.
Heinrichs
M
,
Baumgartner
T
,
Kirschbaum
C
,
Ehlert
U
.
Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2003
;
54
(
12
):
1389
98
.
19.
Eckstein
M
,
Scheele
D
,
Weber
K
,
Stoffel-Wagner
B
,
Maier
W
,
Hurlemann
R
.
Oxytocin facilitates the sensation of social stress
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2014
;
35
(
9
):
4741
50
.
20.
Feldman
R
.
Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans
.
Horm Behav
.
2012
;
61
(
3
):
380
91
.
21.
Scheele
D
,
Wille
A
,
Kendrick
KM
,
Stoffel-Wagner
B
,
Becker
B
,
Güntürkün
O
, et al
.
Oxytocin enhances brain reward system responses in men viewing the face of their female partner
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2013
;
110
(
50
):
20308
13
.
22.
Rilling
JK
,
Young
LJ
.
The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development
.
Science
.
2014
;
345
(
6198
):
771
6
.
23.
Walum
H
,
Young
LJ
.
The neural mechanisms and circuitry of the pair bond
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2018
;
19
(
11
):
643
54
.
24.
Domes
G
,
Heinrichs
M
,
Michel
A
,
Berger
C
,
Herpertz
SC
.
Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2007
;
61
(
6
):
731
3
.
25.
Liu
Y
,
Li
S
,
Lin
W
,
Li
W
,
Yan
X
,
Wang
X
, et al
.
Oxytocin modulates social value representations in the amygdala
.
Nat Neurosci
.
2019
;
22
(
4
):
633
41
.
26.
Baumgartner
T
,
Heinrichs
M
,
Vonlanthen
A
,
Fischbacher
U
,
Fehr
E
.
Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans
.
Neuron
.
2008
;
58
(
4
):
639
50
.
27.
Declerck
CH
,
Boone
C
,
Pauwels
L
,
Vogt
B
,
Fehr
E
.
A registered replication study on oxytocin and trust
.
Nat Hum Behav
.
2020
;
4
(
6
):
646
55
.
28.
Schiller
B
,
Brustkern
J
,
Walker
M
,
Hamm
A
,
Heinrichs
M
.
Oxytocin has sex-specific effects on trust and underlying neurophysiological processes
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2023
;
151
:
106076
.
29.
Lieberz
J
,
Shamay‐Tsoory
SG
,
Saporta
N
,
Esser
T
,
Kuskova
E
,
Stoffel-Wagner
B
, et al
.
Loneliness and the social brain: how perceived social isolation impairs human interactions
.
Adv Sci
.
2021
;
8
(
21
):
2102076
.
30.
Guastella
AJ
,
Howard
AL
,
Dadds
MR
,
Mitchell
P
,
Carson
DS
.
A randomized controlled trial of intranasal oxytocin as an adjunct to exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2009
;
34
(
6
):
917
23
.
31.
Macdonald
K
,
Macdonald
TM
,
Brune
M
,
Lamb
K
,
Wilson
MP
,
Golshan
S
, et al
.
Oxytocin and psychotherapy: a pilot study of its physiological, behavioral and subjective effects in males with depression
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2013
;
38
(
12
):
2831
43
.
32.
Russell
D
,
Peplau
LA
,
Cutrona
CE
.
The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
1980
;
39
(
3
):
472
80
.
33.
Käll
A
,
Shafran
R
,
Lindegaard
T
,
Bennett
S
,
Cooper
Z
,
Coughtrey
A
, et al
.
A common elements approach to the development of a modular cognitive behavioral theory for chronic loneliness
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
2020
;
88
(
3
):
269
82
.
34.
Schwab
R
.
Zur Messung und klinisch-psychologischen Bedeutung von Einsamkeit: Eine Deutschsprachige Version der, Rev. UCLA Loneliness Scale“ von Russel, Peplau und Cutrona
. In:
Schorr
A
, editor.
Bericht über den 13 Kongreß für Angewandte Psychologie
;
1980
. p.
75
9
. Bonn, September 1985. Bonn: Deutschen Psychologen Verband; 1986.
35.
Lee
MR
,
Shnitko
TA
,
Blue
SW
,
Kaucher
AV
,
Winchell
AJ
,
Erikson
DW
, et al
.
Labeled oxytocin administered via the intranasal route reaches the brain in rhesus macaques
.
Nat Commun
.
2020
;
11
(
1
):
2783
.
36.
Spengler
FB
,
Schultz
J
,
Scheele
D
,
Essel
M
,
Maier
W
,
Heinrichs
M
, et al
.
Kinetics and dose dependency of intranasal oxytocin effects on amygdala reactivity
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2017
;
82
(
12
):
885
94
.
37.
Sheehan
DV
,
Lecrubier
Y
,
Sheehan
KH
,
Amorim
P
,
Janavs
J
,
Weiller
E
, et al
.
The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10
.
J Clin Psychiatry
.
1998
;
59
(
Suppl 20
):
22
57
.
38.
Topp
CW
,
Østergaard
SD
,
Søndergaard
S
,
Bech
P
.
The WHO-5 Well-Being Index: a systematic review of the literature
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2015
;
84
(
3
):
167
76
.
39.
Cohen
S
,
Kamarck
T
,
Mermelstein
R
.
A global measure of perceived stress
.
J Health Soc Behav
.
1983
;
24
(
4
):
385
96
.
40.
Klein
EM
,
Brähler
E
,
Dreier
M
,
Reinecke
L
,
Müller
KW
,
Schmutzer
G
, et al
.
The German version of the Perceived Stress Scale–psychometric characteristics in a representative German community sample
.
BMC Psychiatry
.
2016
;
16
:
159
10
.
41.
Bormann
B
,
Burlingame
GM
,
Strauß
B
.
Der gruppenfragebogen (GQ-D)
.
Psychotherapeut
.
2011
;
56
(
4
):
297
309
.
42.
Krogel
J
,
Burlingame
G
,
Chapman
C
,
Renshaw
T
,
Gleave
R
,
Beecher
M
, et al
.
The Group Questionnaire: a clinical and empirically derived measure of group relationship
.
Psychother Res
.
2013
;
23
(
3
):
344
54
.
43.
Bassler
M
,
Potratz
B
,
Krauthauser
H
.
Der “Helping Alliance Questionnaire” (HAQ) von Luborsky. Möglichkeiten zur Evaluation des therapeutischen Prozesses von stationärer Psychotherapie
.
Psychotherapeut
.
1995
;
40
(
1
):
23
32
.
44.
Kühner
C
,
Bürger
C
,
Keller
F
,
Hautzinger
M
.
Reliability and validity of the revised Beck depression inventory (BDI-II) results from German samples
.
Nervenarzt
.
2007
;
78
(
6
):
651
6
.
45.
Liebowitz
MR
.
Social phobia
.
Mod Probl Pharmacopsychiatry
.
1987
;
22
:
141
73
.
46.
Cohen
S
,
Doyle
WJ
,
Skoner
DP
,
Rabin
BS
,
Gwaltney
JM
.
Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold
.
JAMA
.
1997
;
277
(
24
):
1940
4
.
47.
Bernstein
DP
,
Fink
L
,
Handelsman
L
,
Foote
J
,
Lovejoy
M
,
Wenzel
K
, et al
.
Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
1994
;
151
(
8
):
1132
6
.
48.
Schneibel
R
,
Wilbertz
G
,
Scholz
C
,
Becker
M
,
Brakemeier
EL
,
Bschor
T
, et al
.
Adverse events of group psychotherapy in the in‐patient setting–results of a naturalistic trial
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand
.
2017
;
136
(
3
):
247
58
.
49.
Linden
M
,
Strauß
B
,
Scholten
S
,
Nestoriuc
Y
,
Brakemeier
EL
,
Wasilewski
J
.
Definition und Entscheidungsschritte in der Bestimmung und Erfassung von Nebenwirkungen von Psychotherapie
.
Psychother Psych Med
.
2018
;
68
(
9-10
):
377
82
.
50.
Scheele
D
,
Kendrick
KM
,
Khouri
C
,
Kretzer
E
,
Schlapfer
TE
,
Stoffel-Wagner
B
, et al
.
An oxytocin-induced facilitation of neural and emotional responses to social touch correlates inversely with autism traits
.
Neuropsychopharmacology
.
2014
;
39
(
9
):
2078
85
.
51.
Hurlemann
R
,
Scheele
D
.
Dissecting the role of oxytocin in the formation and loss of social relationships
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2016
;
79
(
3
):
185
93
.
52.
Park
C
,
Majeed
A
,
Gill
H
,
Tamura
J
,
Ho
RC
,
Mansur
RB
, et al
.
The effect of loneliness on distinct health outcomes: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2020
;
294
:
113514
.
53.
Hsiao
FH
,
Jow
GM
,
Lai
YM
,
Chen
YT
,
Wang
KC
,
Ng
SM
, et al
.
The long-term effects of psychotherapy added to pharmacotherapy on morning to evening diurnal cortisol patterns in outpatients with major depression
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2011
;
80
(
3
):
166
72
.
54.
Kosfeld
M
,
Heinrichs
M
,
Zak
PJ
,
Fischbacher
U
,
Fehr
E
.
Oxytocin increases trust in humans
.
Nature
.
2005
;
435
(
7042
):
673
6
.
55.
Saporta
N
,
Scheele
D
,
Lieberz
J
,
Nevat
M
,
Kanterman
A
,
Hurlemann
R
, et al
.
Altered activation in the action observation system during synchronization in high loneliness individuals
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2022
;
33
(
2
):
385
402
.
56.
Thorson
KR
,
McKernan
SM
,
West
TV
,
Woolley
JD
,
Mendes
WB
,
Stauffer
CS
.
Oxytocin increases physiological linkage during group therapy for methamphetamine use disorder: a randomized clinical trial
.
Sci Rep
.
2021
;
11
(
1
):
21004
.
57.
Zilcha-Mano
S
,
Goldstein
P
,
Dolev-Amit
T
,
Shamay-Tsoory
S
.
Oxytocin synchrony between patients and therapists as a mechanism underlying effective psychotherapy for depression
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
2021
;
89
(
1
):
49
57
.
58.
Zilcha-Mano
S
,
Porat
Y
,
Dolev
T
,
Shamay-Tsoory
S
.
Oxytocin as a neurobiological marker of ruptures in the working alliance
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2018
;
87
(
2
):
126
7
.