Introduction: Clinical and laboratory studies demonstrate that placebo and nocebo effects influence various symptoms and conditions after the administration of both inert and active treatments. Objective: There is an increasing need for up-to-date recommendations on how to inform patients about placebo and nocebo effects in clinical practice and train clinicians how to disclose this information. Methods: Based on previous clinical recommendations concerning placebo and nocebo effects, a 3-step, invitation-only Delphi study was conducted among an interdisciplinary group of internationally recognized experts. The study consisted of open- and closed-ended survey questions followed by a final expert meeting. The surveys were subdivided into 3 parts: (1) informing patients about placebo effects, (2) informing patients about nocebo effects, and (3) training clinicians how to communicate this information to the patients. Results: There was consensus that communicating general information about placebo and nocebo effects to patients (e.g., explaining their role in treatment) could be beneficial, but that such information needs to be adjusted to match the specific clinical context (e.g., condition and treatment). Experts also agreed that training clinicians to communicate about placebo and nocebo effects should be a regular and integrated part of medical education that makes use of multiple formats, including face-to-face and online modalities. Conclusions: The current 3-step Delphi study provides consensus-based recommendations and practical considerations for disclosures about placebo and nocebo effects in clinical practice. Future research is needed on how to optimally tailor information to specific clinical conditions and patients’ needs, and on developing standardized disclosure training modules for clinicians.

Placebo and nocebo effects can substantially modulate the efficacy and tolerability of medical and psychological interventions for many symptoms and conditions [1-3]. Placebo and nocebo effects refer to favorable or adverse effects, respectively, that can arise as part of an active or inert intervention due to factors such as what the patient expects, the patient-clinician relationship, and other contextual factors [4-9]. These effects can be clinically meaningful, with effect sizes for some conditions approaching treatment effect sizes [10]. Placebo effects can be shaped by a wide range of factors relating to medical practice, e.g., verbal suggestions made by the clinician, nonverbal cues in the patient-clinician interaction, or situational factors in the health care environment [11-16]. Differing viewpoints as to how these effects should be handled exist, e.g., they are often seen as a nuisance in randomized controlled trials as they complicate the testing of new drugs and therapies [17, 18]. On the other hand, there are potential benefits of utilizing placebo effects to boost treatment effects in clinical practice that have been recognized [16, 19, 20], although there is a variety of proposed approaches. Moreover, some caution in utilizing the mechanisms of placebo effects in clinical practice may be prudent, as unforeseen adverse consequences (e.g., violation of expectation and loss of trust) may occur when effects are inadequately explained or elicited by deception (e.g., [21-23]).

Due to these controversies, only a few ideas have been generated on how knowledge about placebo and nocebo effects should be translated into clinical practice [20]. For example, there are not many national guidelines about the use of placebo effects in medical practice [8], and they typically do not provide concrete clinical recommendations on how to optimize care by maximizing placebo and minimizing nocebo effects in clinical practice [24, 25]. It is also important to clearly distinguish between the deceptive use of inactive placebo treatments in clinical practice, which is not recommended, and the systematic use of the mechanisms underlying placebo and nocebo effects to enhance standard treatments in an open way. When considering the potentially wide application of placebo and nocebo effects across health care, there is a need for recommendations on how to communicate about them (e.g., during patient-clinician interactions) to optimize patient outcomes.

As a preliminary step, we provided consensus on the use of placebo and nocebo effects in clinical practice as part of the first official conference of the Society for Interdisciplinary Placebo Studies (SIPS) (https://www.placebosociety.org) [20]. This paper describes a follow-up to the previous recommendations [20] by collecting expert opinion on what should be communicated to patients about placebo and nocebo effects, and how clinicians should be trained to communicate about these topics in the context of medical patient-clinician interactions.

A modified Delphi study was organized for a panel of interdisciplinary experts by invitation (i.e., the speakers invited to participate at the 2019 SIPS conference) [26-28].

Expert Group

Twenty-seven internationally recognized and interprofessional placebo researchers took part in the panel, 67% of whom worked clinically (39% physicians, 56% psychologists, and 5% other, e.g., acupuncturists). Their backgrounds included anesthesiology, neurology, cognitive neuroscience, primary care, internal medicine, health and medical psychology, clinical psychology, psychological medicine, science communication, sports science, sociology, epidemiology, ethics, and philosophy.

Modified Delphi Study

Input for the Delphi study was derived from the existing literature [20]. An open-ended survey was used to generate content for expert consensus on informing patients about placebo and nocebo effects in clinical practice (e.g., what, when, and how should we communicate about these effects?), and how clinicians should be trained to communicate about placebo and nocebo effects. Answers were transcribed verbatim and aggregated into 158 individual items ranked on a scale of 0–10 (0 = totally disagree; 10 = totally agree) in round 2. Means and SD were calculated for each item. During round 3 (a preconference face-to-face clinical expert meeting, added to facilitate a nuanced discussion of possibly differing opinions on survey items), items with high agreement were discussed as input for the recommendations. More details on the methods used can be found in the online supplementary Material (see www.karger.com/doi/10.1159/000510738 for all online suppl. material).

In the sections below and in Figure 1, we briefly describe the results of the Delphi study. The main recommendations are listed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Main recommendations formulated by the expert group for communicating information about placebo and nocebo effects

Main recommendations formulated by the expert group for communicating information about placebo and nocebo effects
Main recommendations formulated by the expert group for communicating information about placebo and nocebo effects
Fig. 1.

Schematic overview of the considerations and the content of information needed for informing patients about placebo and nocebo effects in clinical practice.

Fig. 1.

Schematic overview of the considerations and the content of information needed for informing patients about placebo and nocebo effects in clinical practice.

Close modal

Informing Patients about Placebo and Nocebo Effects

Based on the broad evidence for placebo and nocebo effects in clinical, neurobiological, and laboratory outcomes [5-7, 19, 22, 29-39], the experts agreed that placebo and nocebo effects should be explained, and that patients should receive at least general information about these effects. Providing an outline of the neurobiological and psychological mechanisms could also be helpful, as could informing patients about the limits of placebo effects (e.g., placebo effects are likely to affect symptoms but not the progression of a disease) [40]. The experts recommended placing several stipulations on informing patients about placebo and nocebo effects, e.g., information must be evidence-based and should not overstate the size of placebo effects. For both placebo and nocebo effects, the experts agreed that the information should be tailored to specific patients, conditions, and circumstances. For nocebo effects, this need was particularly emphasized, as a consequence of a delicate balance between following the ethics guidelines of informed consent (according to which patients need to be fully informed about risks and side effects of treatments as well as the possibility of treatment failures) and simultaneously preventing and reducing nocebo effects as much as possible [40-42]. Moreover, for nocebo effects, the need for informing should be considered, and, when deemed necessary, such disclosures should be made carefully in a manner that does not increase anxiety and is not perceived as blaming patients for negative treatment effects (i.e., side effects). Information should, moreover, be presented in a way that minimizes nocebo effects.

Regarding terminology, it was agreed that using the terms “placebo effect” and “nocebo effect” is acceptable, provided they are explained carefully, and that actual nocebo effects be distinguished from adverse events. When explaining the mechanisms involved, or if information is difficult for a patient to understand, clinicians may consider using slightly different terminology (e.g., classical conditioning or response expectancy). Regarding the manner of informing, no one method (information leaflet, consultation, or online information) was preferred over the others.

Training Clinicians in Communicating about Placebo and Nocebo Effects

Next to substantive information about placebo and nocebo effects (e.g., mechanisms, neurobiological and physiological underpinnings, variations in effect sizes and duration of effects, and that placebo effects can also work when people know about the effect), the experts agreed that clinicians should be taught about the relevant ethical issues concerning placebo and nocebo effects. Training could, moreover, emphasize that different patients might require different information, and that placebo and nocebo effects can operate differently for different conditions. Finally, an emphasis should be placed on what clinicians can do to maximize placebo effects and minimize nocebo effects (i.e., deploying strategies such as optimizing verbal and nonverbal communication [15, 30-33, 43-52]).

As to the content of training, the experts agreed that it may be useful to use both general modules as well as modules relevant to specific medical conditions or specialists. They agreed that medical ethics education regarding placebo and nocebo effects should be a routine part of clinical training. Training should preferably be embedded in a medical school or other standard education. However, the experts concluded during the meeting that more research is needed and that empirical testing of the efficacy of training methods is essential.

More details on the Delphi results can be found in the online supplementary Material.

This paper supplies the most up-to-date consensus-based recommendation for communicating information about placebo and nocebo effects in clinical practice (Table 1). The recommendations can support clinicians in their communications with their patients about placebo and nocebo effects.

The results reflect several established and emerging strands of literature in this area. For example, the need to communicate about side effects in ways that do not induce nocebo effects is gaining more widespread recognition [30, 32, 53-58]. Because of ethical considerations [40, 41], tailoring information seems to be particularly imperative for nocebo effects, especially for those patients who may have a high risk of developing these effects [30-32, 43, 53, 59]. The consensus on communicating information about mechanisms is also reflected in studies showing that understanding the mechanisms of placebo effects may help to maximize these effects in clinical practice [2, 20, 42, 60-62].

The results showed a relatively high agreement among the experts that patients should receive general information about placebo and nocebo effects. This includes consensus that the inherent role of these effects in any treatment should be explained, as should related mechanisms like the patient-clinician relationship, but also that this information needs to be adjusted to the specific context (e.g., the characteristics of the patients, the condition in question, and the treatment). For the latter recommendation, the need for careful consideration by the clinician on when and how to communicate information about placebo and nocebo effects should be recognized. This need is partly reflected in the consensus that, for instance, information should be evidence-based and presented without overstating the scale of placebo effects. Overselling placebo effects may cause a violation of expectations when these are too high, thereby eroding trust in health care professionals, which, in turn, may lead to other negative consequences such as disengagement/nonadherence with care (e.g., [21-23]). It is important to emphasize that placebo effects can help optimize treatment outcomes but that they cannot cure disease.

Moreover, clinicians may want to appraise what information is appropriate for each individual patient, and exercise restraint when the benefit of supplying information to a patient is questionable. Care should be taken to not inadvertently elicit iatrogeny, i.e., unintended negative outcomes due to treatment [63-65]. This is a complex issue, where a difficult balance exists between having to inform a patient of the potential side effects of treatment and not causing too much alarm [65]. When done appropriately, informing patients about placebo and nocebo effects may help enhance naturally occurring placebo effects in clinical practice and can even boost the efficacy of a treatment while simultaneously reducing nocebo effects.

The Delphi method has several advantages: it offers rapid consensus and a wide range of expertise can be included, while a socially desirable response due to group pressure is minimized. However, the methodology is not without limitations [26-28]. For example, the biases of the group (who all focus on studying placebos) may have influenced item selection. Also, some panel members may have been more outspoken than others during group discussions. It was nevertheless possible to reach consensus regarding the main recommendations for informing patients and training health professionals.

The recommendations may not encompass all nuances that are typically found in placebo research. For example, it is recommended that general mechanisms are explained, but no recommendations are provided about interactions of these mechanisms with other treatment factors. Another limitation is that some topics may have been overlooked. Additionally, these guidelines may not be generalizable to nonmedical contexts, such as physical therapy, psychotherapy, or treatments for which the role of placebo and nocebo effects has not yet been illustrated. It should also be noted that the recommendations do not legitimize any form of nonevidence-based treatment, nor are we suggesting that proven therapies should be replaced with placebo treatments [66, 67]. Our method also did not allow us to draw conclusions about specific strategies that can maximize placebo effects and minimize nocebo effects for a range of different conditions and contexts. Although we acknowledge their likely importance, we also did not focus on sociodemographic, cultural, personality, or genetic differences between patients in view of the limited existing evidence for subgroups of placebo and nocebo responders [68-70]. For example, there is emerging evidence that specific genotypes (e.g., [5, 71-73]) may be associated with variation in the scale of placebo and nocebo effects, but there is not yet enough evidence to include these findings in clinical recommendations.

Methodologically well-conducted replication studies that support the external validity of our findings are needed for all these areas. Finally, the consensus described here should only be seen as hypothesis-generating. As a consequence, the experts emphasized the need for research focusing on tailoring information to different patients and contexts, and developing evidence-based methods for training clinicians to communicate about placebo and nocebo effects.

This study, based on a 3-stage Delphi approach, is an important step forward towards consensus-based recommendations for communicating about placebo and nocebo effects in medical practice. The experts agreed that patients should receive general information about the mechanisms and neurobiology of placebo and nocebo effects, but that care should be taken to adjust this information to the specific needs of patients and the treatment context. Multimodal training in communication about placebo and nocebo effects should be a regular and integrated part of medical training for clinicians. The experts acknowledged the need for future research to expand knowledge about how best to provide information to patients, and about how clinicians can communicate about placebo and nocebo effects in the course of a treatment. Finally, implementation strategies should be developed to integrate these recommendations into clinical practice and the routine training and education of clinicians.

We would like to acknowledge the following colleagues of the local organizing team of the SIPS conference for their support in the preparation of the survey and meeting described in this paper: KJ Peerdeman, A Skvortsova, L van Vliet, DS Veldhuijzen, JAB Mink, and AE Breugem.

Ethics approval was not required.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

This report was supported by an ERC Consolidator grant (ERC-2013-CoG 617700 EXPECT HEAL-TH), an NWO Vici grant (No. 45316004), and an NWO Stevin prize, all granted to A.W.M.E. L.Y.A. is supported in part by the Intramural Research Program of the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health. U.B., W.R., and R.K. were funded by the Deutsche For-schungsgemeinschaft (project ID 422744262-TRR 289). The funders had no role in the acquisition or analysis of the data or the content of this article.

A workgroup of the SIPS local organizers and international board members (A.W.M.E., L.C., J.G., K.B.J., J.K., L.V., C.J.B., I.K., and S.H.M.) prepared the initiative for the Delphi survey. A.W.M.E. and S.H.M., together with the other work group members, prepared and analyzed the results of the first and second Delphi surveys, prepared the expert meeting, and wrote the paper. All authors contributed to the Delphi survey and/or expert meeting and provided feedback on drafts of the manuscript.

1.
Damien
J
,
Colloca
L
,
Bellei-Rodriguez
CE
,
Marchand
S
.
Pain modulation: from conditioned pain modulation to placebo and nocebo effects in experimental and clinical pain
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
139
:
255
96
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
2.
Petrie
KJ
,
Rief
W
.
Psychobiological mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects: pathways to improve treatments and reduce side effects
.
Annu Rev Psychol
.
2019
Jan
;
70
(
1
):
599
625
.
[PubMed]
0066-4308
3.
Tekampe
J
,
van Middendorp
H
,
Meeuwis
SH
,
van Leusden
JW
,
Pacheco-López
G
,
Hermus
AR
, et al.
Conditioning immune and endocrine parameters in humans: a systematic review
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2017
;
86
(
2
):
99
107
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
4.
Chavarria
V
,
Vian
J
,
Pereira
C
,
Data-Franco
J
,
Fernandes
BS
,
Berk
M
, et al.
The placebo and nocebo phenomena: their clinical management and impact on treatment outcomes
.
Clin Ther
.
2017
Mar
;
39
(
3
):
477
86
.
[PubMed]
0149-2918
5.
Colagiuri
B
,
Schenk
LA
,
Kessler
MD
,
Dorsey
SG
,
Colloca
L
.
The placebo effect: from concepts to genes
.
Neuroscience
.
2015
Oct
;
307
:
171
90
.
[PubMed]
0306-4522
6.
Kaptchuk
TJ
,
Miller
FG
.
Placebo effects in medicine
.
N Engl J Med
.
2015
Jul
;
373
(
1
):
8
9
.
[PubMed]
0028-4793
7.
Wager
TD
,
Atlas
LY
.
The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2015
Jul
;
16
(
7
):
403
18
.
[PubMed]
1471-003X
8.
Colloca
L
,
Barsky
AJ
.
Placebo and nocebo effects
.
N Engl J Med
.
2020
Feb
;
382
(
6
):
554
61
.
[PubMed]
0028-4793
9.
Schmitz
J
,
Müller
M
,
Stork
J
,
Eichler
I
,
Zöllner
C
,
Flor
H
, et al.
Positive treatment expectancies reduce clinical pain and perceived limitations in movement ability despite increased experimental pain: a randomized controlled trial on sham opioid infusion in patients with chronic back pain
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2019
;
88
(
4
):
203
14
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
10.
Wampold
BE
,
Minami
T
,
Tierney
SC
,
Baskin
TW
,
Bhati
KS
.
The placebo is powerful: estimating placebo effects in medicine and psychotherapy from randomized clinical trials
.
J Clin Psychol
.
2005
Jul
;
61
(
7
):
835
54
.
[PubMed]
0021-9762
11.
Blasini
M
,
Peiris
N
,
Wright
T
,
Colloca
L
.
The role of patient-practitioner relationships in placebo and nocebo phenomena
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
139
:
211
31
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
12.
Kleine-Borgmann
J
,
Bingel
U
.
Nocebo effects: neurobiological mechanisms and strategies for prevention and optimizing treatment
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
138
:
271
83
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
13.
Meissner
K
,
Linde
K
.
Are blue pills better than green? How treatment features modulate placebo effects
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
139
:
357
78
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
14.
Necka
EA
,
Atlas
LY
.
The role of social and interpersonal factors in placebo analgesia
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
138
:
161
79
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
15.
Rossettini
G
,
Carlino
E
,
Testa
M
.
Clinical relevance of contextual factors as triggers of placebo and nocebo effects in musculoskeletal pain
.
BMC Musculoskelet Disord
.
2018
Jan
;
19
(
1
):
27
.
[PubMed]
1471-2474
16.
Zion
SR
,
Crum
AJ
.
Mindsets matter: a new framework for harnessing the placebo effect in modern medicine
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
138
:
137
60
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
17.
Friesen
P
.
Mesmer, the placebo effect, and the efficacy paradox: lessons for evidence based medicine and complementary and alternative medicine
.
Crit Public Health
.
2019
;
29
(
4
):
435
47
. 0958-1596
18.
Vase
L
,
Wartolowska
K
.
Pain, placebo, and test of treatment efficacy: a narrative review
.
Br J Anaesth
.
2019
Aug
;
123
(
2
):
e254
62
.
[PubMed]
0007-0912
19.
Enck
P
,
Bingel
U
,
Schedlowski
M
,
Rief
W
.
The placebo response in medicine: minimize, maximize or personalize?
Nat Rev Drug Discov
.
2013
Mar
;
12
(
3
):
191
204
.
[PubMed]
1474-1776
20.
Evers
AW
,
Colloca
L
,
Blease
C
,
Annoni
M
,
Atlas
LY
,
Benedetti
F
, et al.
Implications of placebo and nocebo effects for clinical practice: expert consensus
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2018
;
87
(
4
):
204
10
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
21.
Colloca
L
,
Schenk
LA
,
Nathan
DE
,
Robinson
OJ
,
Grillon
C
.
When expectancies are violated: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study
.
Clin Pharmacol Ther
.
2019
Dec
;
106
(
6
):
1246
52
.
[PubMed]
0009-9236
22.
Fava
GA
,
Guidi
J
,
Rafanelli
C
,
Rickels
K
.
The clinical inadequacy of the placebo model and the development of an alternative conceptual framework
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2017
;
86
(
6
):
332
40
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
23.
Annoni
M
,
Miller
FG
. Informed consent and the ethics of placebo-based interventions for migraine in clinical practice.
Placebo and nocebo in headache
.
London, Berlin
:
Springer
;
2019
.
24.
American Medical Association[Internet]. Use of placebo in clinical practice [cited 2019]. Available from: https://wwwama-assnorg/delivering-care/ethics/use-placebo-clinical-practice.
25.
der Bundesärztekammer
.
Placebo in der Medizin
.
Dtsch Arztebl
.
2010
;
107
:
B1253
7
.0176-3989
26.
Hasson
F
,
Keeney
S
,
McKenna
H
.
Research guidelines for the Delphi survey technique
.
J Adv Nurs
.
2000
Oct
;
32
(
4
):
1008
15
.
[PubMed]
0309-2402
27.
Linstone
HA
.
The Delphi method: techniques and applications
.
Reading
:
Adison Wesley
;
1979
.
28.
McMillan
SS
,
King
M
,
Tully
MP
.
How to use the nominal group and Delphi techniques
.
Int J Clin Pharm
.
2016
Jun
;
38
(
3
):
655
62
.
[PubMed]
2210-7703
29.
Benedetti
F
.
Understanding the mechanisms in health and disease
.
Oxford
:
University Press Oxford
;
2009
.
30.
Colloca
L
,
Finniss
D
.
Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes
.
JAMA
.
2012
Feb
;
307
(
6
):
567
8
.
[PubMed]
0098-7484
31.
Evers
AW
.
Using the placebo effect: how expectations and learned immune function can optimize dermatological treatments
.
Exp Dermatol
.
2017
Jan
;
26
(
1
):
18
21
.
[PubMed]
0906-6705
32.
Greville-Harris
M
,
Dieppe
P
.
Bad is more powerful than good: the nocebo response in medical consultations
.
Am J Med
.
2015
Feb
;
128
(
2
):
126
9
.
[PubMed]
0002-9343
33.
Klinger
R
,
Colloca
L
,
Bingel
U
,
Flor
H
.
Placebo analgesia: clinical applications
.
Pain
.
2014
Jun
;
155
(
6
):
1055
8
.
[PubMed]
0304-3959
34.
Klinger
R
,
Kothe
R
,
Schmitz
J
,
Kamping
S
,
Flor
H
.
Placebo effects of a sham opioid solution: a randomized controlled study in patients with chronic low back pain
.
Pain
.
2017
Oct
;
158
(
10
):
1893
902
.
[PubMed]
0304-3959
35.
Peerdeman
KJ
,
van Laarhoven
AI
,
Keij
SM
,
Vase
L
,
Rovers
MM
,
Peters
ML
, et al.
Relieving patients’ pain with expectation interventions: a meta-analysis
.
Pain
.
2016
Jun
;
157
(
6
):
1179
91
.
[PubMed]
0304-3959
36.
Petersen
GL
,
Finnerup
NB
,
Colloca
L
,
Amanzio
M
,
Price
DD
,
Jensen
TS
, et al.
The magnitude of nocebo effects in pain: a meta-analysis
.
Pain
.
2014
Aug
;
155
(
8
):
1426
34
.
[PubMed]
0304-3959
37.
Schedlowski
M
,
Enck
P
,
Rief
W
,
Bingel
U
.
Neuro-bio-behavioral mechanisms of placebo and nocebo responses: implications for clinical trials and clinical practice
.
Pharmacol Rev
.
2015
Jul
;
67
(
3
):
697
730
.
[PubMed]
0031-6997
38.
Wolters
F
,
Peerdeman
KJ
,
Evers
AW
.
Placebo and nocebo effects across symptoms: from pain to fatigue, dyspnea, nausea and itch
.
Front Psychiatry
.
2019
Jul
;
10
:
470
.
[PubMed]
1664-0640
39.
Colloca
L
,
Akintola
T
,
Haycock
NR
,
Blasini
M
,
Thomas
S
,
Phillips
J
, et al.
Prior therapeutic experiences, not expectation ratings, predict placebo effects: an experimental study in chronic pain and healthy participants
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2020
Jun
;
•••
:
1
8
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
40.
Friesen
P
.
Placebos as a source of agency: evidence and implications
.
Front Psychiatry
.
2019
Oct
;
10
:
721
.
[PubMed]
1664-0640
41.
Alfano
M
.
Placebo effects and informed consent
.
Am J Bioeth
.
2015
;
15
(
10
):
3
12
.
[PubMed]
1526-5161
42.
Manaï
M
,
van Middendorp
H
,
Veldhuijzen
DS
,
Huizinga
TW
,
Evers
AW
.
How to prevent, minimize, or extinguish nocebo effects in pain: a narrative review on mechanisms, predictors, and interventions
.
Pain Rep
.
2019
Jun
;
4
(
3
):
e699
.
[PubMed]
2471-2531
43.
Charlesworth
JE
,
Petkovic
G
,
Kelley
JM
,
Hunter
M
,
Onakpoya
I
,
Roberts
N
, et al.
Effects of placebos without deception compared with no treatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Evid Based Med
.
2017
May
;
10
(
2
):
97
107
.
[PubMed]
1756-5391
44.
Colloca
L
.
Tell me the truth and I will not be harmed: informed consents and nocebo effects
.
Am J Bioeth
.
2017
Jun
;
17
(
6
):
46
8
.
[PubMed]
1526-5161
45.
Colloca
L
,
Howick
J
.
Placebos without deception: outcomes, mechanisms, and ethics
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
138
:
219
40
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
46.
Daniali
H
,
Flaten
MA
.
A qualitative systematic review of effects of provider characteristics and nonverbal behavior on pain, and placebo and nocebo effects
.
Front Psychiatry
.
2019
Apr
;
10
:
242
.
[PubMed]
1664-0640
47.
Howe
LC
,
Leibowitz
KA
,
Crum
AJ
.
When your doctor “gets it” and “gets you”: the critical role of competence and warmth in the patient-provider interaction
.
Front Psychiatry
.
2019
Jul
;
10
:
475
.
[PubMed]
1664-0640
48.
Howick
J
,
Moscrop
A
,
Mebius
A
,
Fanshawe
TR
,
Lewith
G
,
Bishop
FL
, et al.
Effects of empathic and positive communication in healthcare consultations: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J R Soc Med
.
2018
Jul
;
111
(
7
):
240
52
.
[PubMed]
0141-0768
49.
Kelley
JM
,
Kaptchuk
TJ
,
Cusin
C
,
Lipkin
S
,
Fava
M
.
Open-label placebo for major depressive disorder: a pilot randomized controlled trial
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2012
;
81
(
5
):
312
4
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
50.
Locher
C
,
Frey Nascimento
A
,
Kirsch
I
,
Kossowsky
J
,
Meyer
A
,
Gaab
J
.
Is the rationale more important than deception? A randomized controlled trial of open-label placebo analgesia
.
Pain
.
2017
Dec
;
158
(
12
):
2320
8
.
[PubMed]
0304-3959
51.
Palese
A
,
Rossettini
G
,
Colloca
L
,
Testa
M
.
The impact of contextual factors on nursing outcomes and the role of placebo/nocebo effects: a discussion paper
.
Pain Rep
.
2019
Jun
;
4
(
3
):
e716
.
[PubMed]
2471-2531
52.
Rossettini
G
,
Palese
A
,
Geri
T
,
Fiorio
M
,
Colloca
L
,
Testa
M
.
Physical therapists’ perspectives on using contextual factors in clinical practice: findings from an Italian national survey
.
PLoS One
.
2018
Nov
;
13
(
11
):
e0208159
.
[PubMed]
1932-6203
53.
Blease
C
.
Authorized concealment and authorized deception: well-intended secrets are likely to induce nocebo effects
.
Am J Bioeth
.
2015
;
15
(
10
):
23
5
.
[PubMed]
1526-5161
54.
Crichton
F
,
Petrie
KJ
.
Health complaints and wind turbines: the efficacy of explaining the nocebo response to reduce symptom reporting
.
Environ Res
.
2015
Jul
;
140
:
449
55
.
[PubMed]
0013-9351
55.
Faasse
K
,
Huynh
A
,
Pearson
S
,
Geers
AL
,
Helfer
SG
,
Colagiuri
B
.
The influence of side effect information framing on nocebo effects
.
Ann Behav Med
.
2019
Jun
;
53
(
7
):
621
9
.
[PubMed]
0883-6612
56.
Heisig
SR
,
Shedden-Mora
MC
,
Hidalgo
P
,
Nestoriuc
Y
.
Framing and personalizing informed consent to prevent negative expectations: an experimental pilot study
.
Health Psychol
.
2015
Oct
;
34
(
10
):
1033
7
.
[PubMed]
0278-6133
57.
Howick
J
.
Unethical informed consent caused by overlooking poorly measured nocebo effects
.
J Med Ethics
.
2020
Feb
;
medethics-2019-105903
.
[PubMed]
0306-6800
58.
Pouillon
L
,
Danese
S
,
Hart
A
,
Fiorino
G
,
Argollo
M
,
Selmi
C
, et al.
Consensus report: clinical recommendations for the prevention and management of the nocebo effect in biosimilar-treated IBD patients
.
Aliment Pharmacol Ther
.
2019
May
;
49
(
9
):
1181
7
.
[PubMed]
0269-2813
59.
Svensberg
K
,
Nordeng
H
,
Gaffari
S
,
Faasse
K
,
Horne
R
,
Lupattelli
A
.
Perceived sensitivity to medicines: a study among chronic medicine users in Norway
.
Int J Clin Pharm
.
2019
Jun
;
41
(
3
):
804
12
.
[PubMed]
2210-7703
60.
Blease
C
.
The principle of parity: the ‘placebo effect’ and physician communication
.
J Med Ethics
.
2012
Apr
;
38
(
4
):
199
203
.
[PubMed]
0306-6800
61.
Crum
A
,
Zuckerman
B
.
Changing mindsets to enhance treatment effectiveness
.
JAMA
.
2017
May
;
317
(
20
):
2063
4
.
[PubMed]
0098-7484
62.
Klinger
R
,
Stuhlreyer
J
,
Schwartz
M
,
Schmitz
J
,
Colloca
L
.
Clinical use of placebo effects in patients with pain disorders
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
139
:
107
28
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
63.
Barsky
AJ
.
The iatrogenic potential of the physician’s words
.
JAMA
.
2017
Dec
;
318
(
24
):
2425
6
.
[PubMed]
0098-7484
64.
Bootzin
RR
,
Bailey
ET
.
Understanding placebo, nocebo, and iatrogenic treatment effects
.
J Clin Psychol
.
2005
Jul
;
61
(
7
):
871
80
.
[PubMed]
0021-9762
65.
Fava
GA
,
Rafanelli
C
.
Iatrogenic factors in psychopathology
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2019
;
88
(
3
):
129
40
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
66.
Beedie
C
,
Whyte
G
,
Lane
AM
,
Cohen
E
,
Raglin
J
,
Hurst
P
, et al.
‘Caution, this treatment is a placebo. It might work, but it might not’: why emerging mechanistic evidence for placebo effects does not legitimise complementary and alternative medicines in sport
.
Br J Sports Med
.
2018
Jul
;
52
(
13
):
817
8
.
[PubMed]
0306-3674
67.
Benedetti
F
.
The dangerous side of placebo research: is hard science boosting pseudoscience?
Clin Pharmacol Ther
.
2019
Dec
;
106
(
6
):
1166
8
.
[PubMed]
0009-9236
68.
Friesen
P
,
Blease
C
.
Placebo effects and racial and ethnic health disparities: an unjust and underexplored connection
.
J Med Ethics
.
2018
Nov
;
44
(
11
):
774
81
.
[PubMed]
0306-6800
69.
Geers
AL
,
Helfer
SG
,
Kosbab
K
,
Weiland
PE
,
Landry
SJ
.
Reconsidering the role of personality in placebo effects: dispositional optimism, situational expectations, and the placebo response
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2005
Feb
;
58
(
2
):
121
7
.
[PubMed]
0022-3999
70.
Trivedi
MH
,
South
C
,
Jha
MK
,
Rush
AJ
,
Cao
J
,
Kurian
B
, et al.
A novel strategy to identify placebo responders: prediction index of clinical and biological markers in the EMBARC trial
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2018
;
87
(
5
):
285
95
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
71.
Colloca
L
,
Wang
Y
,
Martinez
PE
,
Chang
YC
,
Ryan
KA
,
Hodgkinson
C
, et al.
OPRM1 rs1799971, COMT rs4680, and FAAH rs324420 genes interact with placebo procedures to induce hypoalgesia
.
Pain
.
2019
Aug
;
160
(
8
):
1824
34
.
[PubMed]
0304-3959
72.
Hall
KT
,
Loscalzo
J
,
Kaptchuk
T
.
Pharmacogenomics and the placebo response
.
ACS Chem Neurosci
.
2018
Apr
;
9
(
4
):
633
5
.
[PubMed]
1948-7193
73.
Hall
KT
,
Loscalzo
J
,
Kaptchuk
TJ
.
Genetics and the placebo effect: the placebome
.
Trends Mol Med
.
2015
May
;
21
(
5
):
285
94
.
[PubMed]
1471-4914
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.