Background: There is consistent evidence showing an interplay between psychological processes and immune function in health and disease processes. Objectives: The present systematic review and meta-analysis aims to provide a concise overview of the effectiveness of stress-reducing psychological interventions on the activation of immune responses in both healthy subjects and patients. Methods: Included are 3 types of challenges: in vivo, in vitro, and psychophysiological. Such challenges are designed to mimic naturally occurring immune-related threats. Results: A systematic literature search was conducted using PubMed, EMBASE, and PsychInfo, resulting in 75 eligible studies. The risk of bias was assessed with the Cochrane risk-of-bias tool. Across all studies, a small-to-medium effect size was found for the effects of psychological interventions on optimization of the immune function (g = 0.33; 95% CI 0.22–0.43). While the largest effects were found for in vivo immune-related challenges (g = 0.61; 95% CI 0.34–0.88; especially on studies that incorporated skin tests and wound healing), studies incorporating psychophysiological challenges and in vitro immune-related stimulations similarly suggest more optimal immune responses among those receiving stress-reducing interventions (g = 0.28; 95% CI 0.15–0.42). Conclusion: These findings showed substantial heterogeneity depending on the type of challenge, the study populations, and the intervention types. These data demonstrate support for the effectiveness of stress-reducing psychological interventions in improving immunity in studies that tested immune function by means of incorporating an in vivo, in vitro, or psychophysiological challenge. Future research should more consistently incorporate challenges into the study design to gather more insights in the mechanisms underlying the optimized immune function following a psychological intervention. This is also relevant for clinical practice, as psychological interventions can possibly supplement, or at least partially replace, current drug treatments in various somatic conditions to reduce side effects.

Psychosocial features can influence clinical outcomes [1-3]. More specifically, stressful events can influence the functioning of the immune system [4-6]. Several systematic reviews and meta-analyses have overall shown that particularly chronic stress suppresses protective immune responses and promotes pathological immune responses, including inflammatory responses [7-12]. Moreover, stress-related disorders, including anxiety and depression, also turned out to be associated with affective-related deficits through immune alterations [13]. These immune alterations can be expressed as slower wound healing [8, 9], impaired responses to vaccines [7], and progression of infectious and immune-mediated diseases [7, 14, 15].

Various psychological interventions, including cognitive behavioral therapy [16], mindfulness [17-21], and relaxation [17], have been found to effectively reduce stress. Therefore, it has been argued that such stress-reducing interventions may help to counteract the adverse effects of stress on immune functioning. A previous meta-analysis, however, found little support for an immune-optimizing potential of psychological interventions [22]. Some supporting evidence was provided by studies using conditioning and hypnosis interventions, although the results were heterogeneous. Due to substantial variation in immune outcomes, the generalizability was uncertain [22]. More specifically, the immune outcomes in these studies varied from counting white blood cell subsets to evaluating cell function by activating the immune system by either in vitro (i.e., exposing isolated white blood cells to an immune-activating stimulus) or in vivo (i.e., stimulating an immune response in the intact person; e.g., vaccination) methods. Each of these methods provides a different window and type of information on the functioning of the immune system. Counting cells in a resting state provides information on the number of immune cells in the circulation. However, the circulation represents only a small and selective proportion of the total cell population, it is highly dynamic within individuals, and the normal range of adequate cell numbers is rather broad. Therefore, in somatically healthy participants cell counts are of uncertain clinical significance. On the other hand, the response of the immune system to activating stimuli is considered a more representative estimate of a person’s ability to mount an adequate immune response in the face of a natural challenge and may be considered a more biologically valid marker of immunocompetence [23].

In vitro activations include natural killer cell activity (NKCA), a stimulated lymphocyte proliferation response (LPR), and stimulated proinflammatory and anti-inflammatory cytokine production (i.e., chemical challenges), whereas in vivo stimulations include hypersensitivity responses to skin tests, the time of healing of a biopsy wound, or the extent to which a vaccine produces antibodies (i.e., physical challenges). In addition to the above-mentioned in vitroand in vivoactivations of the immune system, psychosocial stress can also challenge the immune system [4, 5, 24, 25]. Therefore, a number of studies have evoked psychosocial stress in their participants by exposing them to psychophysiological challenges, i.e., challenges that have the potential to evoke a psychophysiological stress response, including exposure to a psychosocial stress task, to obtain additional information on how stress-reducing psychological interventions may optimize the extent to which the immune system responds to these challenges [26]. A recent systematic review provided support for the effectiveness of psychological interventions in optimization of wound healing [27]. There is, however, no recent examination of the effectiveness of stress-reducing interventions on a broader range of immune challenges, also taking psychophysiological challenges into account.

In the last few decades, studies have evaluated how the immune system responds to chemical, physical, and psychophysiological challenges after undergoing a stress-reducing psychological intervention. Previous systematic reviews and meta-analyses, including the systematic review and meta-analysis of Miller and Cohen [22] in the previous century, have already summarized the effectiveness of psychological interventions on immune function [20, 27]. This study incorporated various outcome measures that were assessed during resting states or after a challenge. However, the focus of this previous study was not on challenges per se and it did not assess the immune response after psychophysiological challenges. It can be presumed that exposing participants to different kind of challenges, i.e., chemical, physical, or psychophysiological, provides more insights in the actual responsiveness of the immune system to a natural challenge compared to assessing resting state outcomes. Therefore, the aim of the current systematic review and meta-analysis is to summarize the effectiveness of stress-reducing psychological interventions directed at optimizing immune function, focusing on studies incorporating various in vivo or in vitro immune-related and/or psychophysiological stimulations/challenges into the study design. We expected that after a stress-reducing psychological intervention participants would show a more optimized immune response to challenges as compared to participants who did not receive a stress-reducing psychological intervention. More specifically, after the stress-reducing psychological intervention we expected a higher NKCA, higher anti-inflammatory cytokine responses, lower proinflammatory cytokine responses, higher LPR, higher antibody responses, and higher delayed-type hypersensitivity responses, as well as faster wound healing. We analyzed the pooled effects of the 3 types of challenges together as well as separately.

This systematic review and meta-analysis was performed according to PRISMA criteria [28] and it was registered in PROSPERO (registration No. CRD42017055722).

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Studies were included when they met the following inclusion criteria according to PICO criteria [29]: (P) incorporation of human participants (patients or healthy participants); (I) a stress-reducing psychological intervention (which was defined as having cognitive behavior change techniques [30] as the main component, i.e., duration of more than 50% of the intervention time, such as psychotherapy, mindfulness, or relaxation) – interventions that combined psychological intervention components with physical intervention components were only included when the focus of the intervention was on the psychological components, i.e., more than 50% of the intervention; (C) incorporation of at least 1 control group without a stress-reducing psychological intervention; and (O) inclusion of immune outcome measures assessed in blood or saliva (e.g., quantification of cytokines and lymphocytes) as well as incorporation of immune-related and/or psychophysiological challenges into the study design which were assessed after the start of the stress-reducing psychological intervention. Articles were excluded when they assessed immunological functioning not by objective measurements or parameters, but when they were, for example, solely based on self-reports (e.g., self-reported infection), when they were based on case studies, or when they had insufficient methodological or statistical details about the immune or psychophysiological challenges or results (e.g., conference abstracts).

Literature Search Strategy

A systematic search was conducted using the databases PubMed, EMBASE, and PsychInfo until January 26, 2017. The search terms included Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) and words from title/abstract (tiab) as qualifiers, classified in 3 categories: stress-reducing psychological interventions, immune function, and immune-related as well as psychophysiological challenges (for the search strategy per database, see online suppl. Table 1; for all online suppl. material, see www.karger.com/doi/10.1159/000501645). All retrieved references were loaded into Endnote and 2 independent reviewers (L.S. and P.C.) screened the titles, abstracts, and subsequently full texts when appropriate regarding study eligibility and relevance. The reference lists of the included studies were additionally searched for potential eligible studies.

Data Extraction

A data extraction form was used to extract relevant data from the eligible studies. The extracted information for each study included: study population (e.g., healthy participants or patients); participant demographics; details of the intervention and control conditions; study methodology; incorporated chemical, physical, and/or psychophysiological challenges; immune outcome parameters; relevant outcome data; statistical analyses; and relevant information concerning the methodological quality assessment. The information was extracted by the 2 reviewers (L.S. and P.I.C.) independently. Discrepancies were identified and resolved through discussion by involving one or more additional reviewer(s) (D.S.V., J.A.B., and A.W.M.E.).

Methodological Quality Assessment in the Included Studies

Two reviewers (L.S. and P.I.C.) furthermore independently assessed the risk of bias (RoB) of the included studies using the Cochrane RoB tool [31]. The biases that were assessed included: selection bias (process of randomization and concealment of allocation), performance bias (blinding of participants and research personnel), detection bias (blinding of outcome assessment), reporting bias (handling of missing data), and attrition bias (description of reasons for withdrawal in all conditions). Biases were classified as being low, high, or unclear. Disagreements between the review authors regarding the RoB in particular studies were resolved by discussion, with involvement of a third review author (D.S.V.) if necessary.

Data Analyses

Data were analyzed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software version 3.3.070 (Biostat, Englewood, CO, USA). Hedges’ g was the effect size metric that was applied in the descriptive statistics of this study. The effect size was calculated by subtracting the pre- from the post-immune outcome parameters in the control group and subsequently subtracting this difference score from the difference score in the intervention group, divided by the pooled SD and weighted across the number of subjects in each group. Effect sizes of 0.2 can be considered small, whereas 0.5 and 0.8 can be considered medium and large, respectively [32]. For the included studies performing within-subjects comparisons, the pre-post correlation coefficient could not be derived and therefore a correlation coefficient of r = 0.05 was imputed. In case a study containing multiple conditions with eligible psychological interventions, these groups were combined into a single pairwise comparison, according to the recommendations of the Cochrane handbook [31]. The pooled effects were analyzed using a random-effects model. Heterogeneity was assessed by evaluating the I2statistic and by visual inspection of the forest plot. Values of I2= 25, 50, and 75% can be interpreted as low, moderate, and high heterogeneity, respectively. In cases in which the results of a study were based on postintervention scores only (e.g., in the case of wound healing studies), the effect size was based on the postintervention scores. When the descriptive statistics were not available authors were requested to provide those data, and when the data were not provided alternative methods were used to calculate the effect size (e.g., using reported statistics, reported mean change scores, etc.). When studies reported that the results were not significant, without further specification of the outcomes, effect sizes were computed assuming no differences between the groups (r = 0.00). Because this is a rather conservative strategy that had to be applied to a substantial proportion of the data (i.e., imputation was used in 23.8% of the cases), meta-analyses were performed with and without those studies in order to evaluate the potential bias of this method. All immune outcomes were scaled in the direction of positive Hedges’ g representing an optimized immune function. More specifically, a higher NKCA, higher anti-inflammatory cytokine responses, lower proinflammatory cytokine responses, higher LPR, higher antibody responses, and higher delayed-type hypersensitivity responses, as well as faster wound healing, were interpreted as optimized immune outcomes.

The pooled effects of all 3 different types of challenges (i.e., in vitro immune-related stimulations, in vivo immune-related challenges, and psychophysiological challenges) were analyzed together and separately. The in vitro immune-related stimulations were subsequently subcategorized into NKCA, stimulated LPR, and stimulated cytokine production. In vivo immune-related challenges were subdivided into wound healing, vaccine responses, and immediate as well as delayed-type hypersensitivity responses after skin tests.In vivo psychophysiological challenges were further subdivided into acute and more protracted stress challenges, separately for plasma numbers of lymphocytes (i.e., enumeration of CD4, CD8, and CD56 numbers) and cytokines (i.e., quantification of IL-1β, IL-6, IL-8, and TNF-α). When the outcomes of in vitro stimulations were assessed on multiple concentrations of the stimulus (e.g., multiple effector-to-target ratios to evaluate NKCA or various dilutions to evaluate LPR), the effect size was derived from the concentration that most optimally differentiated conditions (i.e., the stimulus concentrations that showed the largest differences). Planned subset analyses evaluated the effects of different types of challenges within a specific category.

Data of at least 3 studies had to be available in order to conduct a meta-analysis. Sensitivity analyses were performed concerning the reliability of the results in that it was investigated whether the results would remain comparable when taking RoB and publication bias into account. In order to assess the stability of the overall effect size, it was investigated whether the effects were similar when studies with a substantial RoB (i.e., studies containing at least 1 classification of high RoB) were excluded from the analyses. In addition, publication bias was assessed by inspection of the funnel plot and applying the trim-and-fill method of Duval and Tweedie [33].

Search Results

Online suppl. Figure 1 shows the flow chart of the systematic search and study selection. A total of 19,780 studies (including duplicates) were found by searching PubMed, EMBASE, and PsychInfo. After removing duplicates and screening the studies on title and abstract, 138 articles were examined in full text by the 2 independent reviewers. Of those, 65 articles fulfilled the inclusion criteria. Screening of the reference lists of the included articles yielded 9 additional eligible studies, which were not identified in the primary search as most of these studies did not specify immune outcome measures in the title and/or abstract. In total, 75 studies reported in 74 articles were included.

Study Characteristics

A total of 4,141 participants took part in the 75 studies. Detailed information concerning the study characteristics and incorporated psychological interventions are described in online suppl. Table 2. The total individual study sample size varied between 12 [34] and 252 subjects [35] (mean = 57, SD = 48). In 29 studies (38.7%), healthy volunteers were included as the study population [34, 36-62]. Other samples included patients or vulnerable adults, e.g., patients with various types of cancer [63-82], patients with HIV infection [35, 83-87], patients with rheumatoid arthritis [26, 88-90], older adults [91-94], patients with asthma/allergies [95-97], widows/women who had lost a close relative to cancer [98, 99], patients with ulcerative colitis [100, 101], women with depression after bypass surgery [102], patients with late-life insomnia [103], women suffering from infertility [104], veterans [105], and patients who had undergone surgery [106]. The mean age of the participants varied between 18.5 and 78.8 years. Details on age were not provided in 7 studies (9.3%). Twenty-four studies (32.0%) only included female participants, whereas 9 studies (12.0%) only included male participants. In 36 studies (48.0%), both males and females were included. Details on gender were not reported in 6 studies (8.0%).

RoB Assessment

Online suppl. Figure 2 presents the RoB graph and online suppl. Figure 3 the RoB summary. Of the 75 studies, 68 (90.7%) did not provide sufficient details on the methods used to randomize participants and 71 articles (94.7%) did not sufficiently specify the methods of allocation concealment (unclear RoB). RoB on performance was low for 2 articles (2.7%) due to adequate blinding procedures. In 9 articles (12.0%), participants and/or personnel were aware of the group allocation, which could have led to performance bias (high RoB). For 26 articles (34.7%), the RoB concerning a lack of blinding of participants and personnel was low. In 35 articles (46.7%), the drop-out rates and reasons for drop-out were sufficiently described and unrelated to the study outcomes, which resulted in a low RoB evaluation regarding incomplete outcome data. No study protocol was available for 73 articles (96.1%), resulting in an unclear RoB regarding selective reporting.

Type of Stress-Reducing Psychological Interventions

In total, 82 stress-reducing psychological interventions were evaluated in the 75 studies. Most interventions (28 interventions; 34.1%) were based on relaxation or stress management. Multicomponent cognitive-behavioral interventions, including psycho-education and various cognitive and behavioral techniques, were also common and assessed in 18 cases (22.0%). Other interventions were based on manualized mindfulness and/or meditation (13 interventions; 15.9%), hypnosis (12 interventions; 14.6%), emotional disclosure (7 interventions; 8.5%), and counseling (4 interventions; 4.9%). The interventions varied in their total duration from a single session to multiple sessions over a period of 12 months.

Regarding the guidance of the interventions, all interventions included face-to-face or telephone appointments, except for 2 interventions that relied on self-practice. Of the guided interventions, 48 (58.5%) also encouraged self-practice.

Overall Immune Effects

Detailed information concerning the immune-related challenges and outcomes for each study is presented in online suppl. Table 3.

When performing an overall random-effects meta-analysis on the data, i.e., irrespectively of the incorporated challenge, an overall small effect size was found (k =84, g = 0.33; 95% CI 0.22–0.43), with moderate heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 59.41%). When excluding the studies that were set at r = 0.00, a slightly higher overall small effect size was found (k = 64, g = 0.43; 95% CI 0.30–0.55, I2 = 67.69%).

Exploratory Analyses for Participants with and without Somatic Conditions

For studies that incorporated patients with somatic conditions, a small overall effect size was found (k = 40, g = 0.34; 95% CI 0.17–0.52), with moderate heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 71.94%).

For studies that incorporated participants without somatic conditions, also a small overall effect size was found (k = 44, g = 0.31; 95% CI 0.20–0.43), with low heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 34.10%).

In vitro Immune-Related Stimulations

Of the 75 studies, 52 (68.4%) incorporated at least 1 in vitro immune stimulation test, including NKCA (32 studies), LPR (28 studies), cytokine production (10 studies), and monocyte chemotaxis (1 study).

Online suppl. Figure 4 presents the forest plot on the random-effects meta-analysis for in vitro immune-related stimulations. Overall, a small effect size was found (k = 52, g = 0.28; 95% CI 0.15–0.42), with moderate heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 61.43%). After excluding the studies that were set at r = 0.00, a small effect size was found (k = 39, g = 0.39; 95% CI 0.22–0.56, I2 = 70.75%). When looking at specific subgroups of in vitro immune stimulation tests, we found a small effect size for NKCA (k =31, g = 0.21; 95% CI 0.06–0.35, I2 =40.22%), LPR (k =28, g = 0.35; 95% CI 0.13–0.57, I2 = 73.07%), and cytokine production (k =9, g = 0.32; 95% CI 0.14–0.51, I2 < 0.01%).

Exploratory Analyses for Participants with and without Somatic Conditions

For studies that incorporated patients with somatic conditions, a small effect size was found (k =33, g = 0.28; 95% CI 0.10–0.46), with moderate heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 69.54%).

For studies that incorporated participants without somatic conditions, also a small effect size was found (k = 19, g = 0.28; 95% CI 0.08–0.48), with low heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 33.76%).

In vivo Immune-Related Challenges

In vivo immune-related challenges, including skin testing (8 studies), vaccination (5 studies), and wound healing (4 studies), were incorporated into the study designs of 17 studies (22.4%).

Online suppl. Figure 5 presents the results of the random-effects meta-analysis on the pooled effects of in vivo immune-related challenges. A medium effect size was found (k =17, g = 0.61; 95% CI 0.34–0.88), with high heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 74.59%). After excluding the studies that were set at r = 0.00, a similar medium effect size was found (k = 15, g = 0.64; 95% CI 0.35–0.92, I2 = 76.73%).When looking at specific subgroups within the in vivo immune-related challenges, a large effect size was found for studies using skin tests (k = 8, g = 0.80; 95% CI 0.30–1.30, I2 =80.72%). Furthermore, a small effect size was found for vaccine studies (k =5, g = 0.37; 95% CI –0.17 to 0.90, I2 = 77.69), and a medium effect size was found for wound healing studies (k =4, g = 0.75; 95% CI 0.45–1.05, I2 < 0.01%).

Exploratory Analyses for Participants with and without Somatic Conditions

For studies that incorporated patients with somatic conditions, a high effect size was found (k = 4, g = 1.5; 95% CI 0.4–2.7), with high heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 86.973%).

For studies that incorporated participants without somatic conditions, a medium effect size was found (k = 17, g = 0.61; 95% CI 0.34–0.88), with moderate heterogeneity across the studies (I2 = 74.59%).

Most studies were based on skin testing. Of the 4 studies that included patients with somatic conditions, 3 studies included allergic patients who were exposed to skin tests, and yielded high effect sizes (k =3, g = 2.02; 95% CI –0.03–4.06). Five studies were found that included participants without somatic conditions. When these study findings were compared to the patients with somatic conditions, small effect sizes were found (k = 5, g = 0.28; 95% CI 0.05–0.51).

Psychophysiological Challenges

In 16 studies (19.7%), a psychophysiological challenge was incorporated; acute challenges included a speech task, exams, a cold pressor test, and a treadmill exercise test (10 studies), and challenges of a more protracted character, including academic stress and HIV serostatus notification (6 studies).

In online suppl. Figure 6, the results of the random-effects meta-analysis on the pooled effects of psychophysiological challenges is shown. One study was not included in the meta-analysis as the outcomes of that study were not based on plasma measurements, T-cell enumeration, or cytokine quantification. Overall, no effect was found (k = 15, g = 0.18; 95% CI 0.01–0.35, I2 < 0.01), whereas a small effect size was found when excluding the studies that were set at r = 0.00 (k = 10, g = 0.28; 95% CI 0.07–0.49, I2 < 0.01).When assessing studies that incorporated enumeration of lymphocyte subsets after a psychophysiological challenge (i.e., CD4, CD8, and CD56), a small effect size was found for studies incorporating a more protracted stress challenge (k = 4, g = 0.33; 95% CI = –0.06 to 0.72, I2 = 1.68%). For acute stress challenges, there were not enough studies available that had incorporated those markers in order to evaluate the effects after an acute stress challenge (k = 2). For studies that incorporated plasma cytokine measurements (i.e., IL-1β, IL-6, IL-8, and TNF-α) after a psychophysiological challenge, a small effect size was described in studies incorporating an acute challenge (k = 4, g = 0.22; 95% CI –0.04 to 0.49, I2 < 0.01%), whereas no studies incorporated those markers to evaluate the effects after a more protracted stress challenge.

Exploratory Analyses for Participants with and without Somatic Conditions

For studies that incorporated patients with somatic conditions, no effect was found (k = 3, g = 0.11; 95% CI –0.21 to 0.42), with low heterogeneity across the studies (I2 < 0.01%).

For studies that incorporated participants without somatic conditions, also no effect was found (k = 12, g = 0.22; 95% CI 0.01–0.42), with low heterogeneity across the studies (I2 < 0.01%).

Sensitivity Analyses

RoB within Studies

When studies with a presumed high RoB were excluded from the analyses, 23 of 84 outcomes were excluded. However, the overall effect size was not substantially altered (k = 61, g = 0.34; 95% CI 0.20–0.48).

Publication Bias

The funnel plot is displayed in online suppl. Figure 7 and suggests the presence of publication bias. The trim-and-fill method indicates that 12 studies were expected to be missing with below-average effects, as indicated by the black dots. When imputing those studies, the effect size decreased to g = 0.21 (95% CI 0.09–0.32).

Over the last few decades, studies have evaluated the effectiveness of stress-reducing psychological interventions on immune function by incorporating chemical, physical, and psychophysiological challenges into the study design. These challenges are thought to present a biologically more valid reflection on the effectiveness of stress-reducing psychological interventions in optimization of the immune function as compared to unstimulated quantitative immune outcomes [23, 107, 108]. The present systematic review and meta-analysis summarized immune-related outcomes after a chemical, physical, or psychophysiological challenge following a stress-reducing psychological intervention in both healthy subjects and patients.

Overall, the findings demonstrated a small (heterogeneous) positive effect size for optimization of the immune function. As a conservative method was applied to handle studies that reported no significant results without further specifying the actual group differences, the overall effect size possibly represents a slightly underestimated effect size. While the largest effects were found for in vivo immune-related challenges (especially in studies that incorporated skin tests and wound healing), studies incorporating psychophysiological challenges and in vitro immune-related stimulations similarly suggest more optimal immune responses among those receiving stress-reducing interventions.

When focusing on in vitroimmune-related stimulations, small effect sizes were found. Studies were highly diverse regarding the source of material and technical details of the stimulation. For example, studies varied in the target of stimulation (e.g., stimulation of T cells and NK cells), the types of outcomes (e.g., proliferation, cytokine production, and killing monocytes) and the types of concentrations and the duration of stimuli. Likewise, a subset of studies stimulated whole blood, thereby performing tests in a biologically normal blood-plasma context, whereas others stimulated peripheral blood mononuclear cells, whereby tests are performed in artificial buffer solutions. Therefore, whole blood stimulations comprise a rather diverse range of cell populations (e.g., neutrophils, eosinophils, etc.), whereas the cell populations in peripheral blood mononuclear cells are more well-defined, resulting in different environments of stimulation. In addition, important details such as the concentrations used or which type of immune cells were stimulated, were often lacking from the Methods section, while such aspects may substantially influence the results. Future studies are therefore encouraged to report more carefully on the methodological details. This could, for example, be acquired by applying a standard format for reporting the methodology, such as the Minimum Information About a Microarray Experiment (MIAME) guidelines [109] or the Minimal Information About T cell Assays (MIATA) standard [110]. In addition, since in vitro stimulations are applied outside the body, those challenges may comprise a less biologically relevant valid representation of real-life immune threats as compared to in vivo challenges, although in vitro immune-related stimulations are easier to implement into the study design.

When focusing on in vivo immune-related challenges, studies on skin tests and wound healing found largest effect sizes and were mostly based on evaluating wound size alteration instead of quantitative immune outcome measures. These outcome parameters contain a rather unidirectional and straightforward representation of immune function (i.e., faster wound healing represents a more optimal immune response). Thus, of all of the immune-related challenges examined, the most convincing evidence was found for stress-reducing psychological interventions optimizing the immune performance in cases of wound healing (medium effect size) and skin-based tests (high effect size). Even though these immune-related challenges probably represent a general stimulation of the immune performance, this could imply that stress-reducing interventions could be particularly clinically relevant for patients with immune-related skin conditions, such as patients recovering from inflammation-sensitive surgical wounds. Contrary to these findings, only a small effect size was found for vaccines. Due to the small number of studies that incorporated a vaccine (5 studies), and variation in the type of incorporated vaccines and the included time points (influenza vaccines, but also 1 study with a hepatitis B vaccine incorporating various measurement points), the present meta-analysis could not provide a conclusive view on this subcategory ofin vivoimmune-related challenges. As few studies incorporated a vaccine, future research would be helpful to further elucidate the effects of psychological interventions on in vivo immune-related challenges, particularly in the area of vaccination and related immune outcomes.

For studies incorporating psychophysiological challenges, small effect sizes on immune measures were found when incorporating acute challenges (e.g., exam stress), and small effect sizes were found when incorporating chronic stressors (e.g., academic stress). Although the data did not seem to display a high statistical heterogeneity, the incorporated challenges and immune outcome parameters were highly diverse across studies. More specifically, studies included acute challenges such as exams, speech tasks (some accompanied with or without a mental arithmetic task), a treadmill exercise test, and a cold pressor task, as well as more protracted stress challenges such as serostatus notification for individuals undergoing HIV testing and academic stress experienced by students during an examination period. Since the findings of the present study were based on a small number of studies with mostly limited ecological validity of the stressors, i.e., only some included challenges represented chronic stress as experienced by people in daily life, future work should focus on incorporating stressors with a high external validity (e.g., social-evaluative stressors for socially anxious subjects or more daily-life chronic stress such as rumination) in order to evaluate the effects of psychological interventions on immune function [5].

Most the studies that incorporated psychophysiological challenges involved healthy participants (14 out of 17 studies). As healthy participants are supposed to have a well-functioning immune system, they are expected to show responses within the normal range to standard immune system challenges also in absence of a stress-reducing psychological intervention [5]. The challenging situation to which these healthy participants are exposed, therefore, must be powerful enough to detect any relevant alterations in immune function in response to a psychological intervention. It is possible that combination of a psychophysiological challenge with an in vivo immune-related challenge can boost the effects of the separate challenges and possibly provide healthy participants with a more robust immune system challenge. Only 1 study in the present systematic review and meta-analysis combined an in vivo immune-related challenge, i.e., suction blisters on the volar forearm, with a psychophysiological challenge, i.e., a Trier social stress test, to evaluate the effects of a stress-reducing psychological intervention [55]. In that study, participants who received a stress-reducing mindfulness intervention showed a lower post-stress (i.e., after the Trier social stress test) inflammatory response to the in vivoimmune-related and psychophysiological challenges compared to a control group that received a control health enhancement program. The incorporation of both an in vivo immune-related challenge and a psychophysiological challenge provides a more elaborate view of the underlying processes of immune function after a psychological intervention, i.e., evaluating immune function after activation of the immune system through different challenges that can boost each other’s effectiveness. Future studies may consider incorporating multiple challenges into their design when examining immune function in healthy participants in order to hypothetically provide them with a rather robust challenge [111].

Regarding the effective components of stress-reducing psychological interventions, no strong conclusions can be drawn at this point due to the substantial heterogeneity in the incorporated intervention elements across studies, including the duration and number of sessions, the intervention target, and ways of guidance (e.g., self-practice, structured guided sessions, etc.). An exploratory evaluation of the data, however, showed that multiple studies explored the role of self-practice during the intervention (e.g., completing homework assignments) for immune outcomes [36, 38, 41, 46, 47, 52, 55, 59, 68, 75, 78, 106]. Most of those studies found a positive association between the frequency of self-practice and optimized immune outcomes [36, 46, 47, 52, 55, 68, 78]. Although we could not formally test this observation in our meta-analysis due to substantial heterogeneity in study designs (e.g., selection of immune outcomes and differences in level of details concerning the specification of self-practice frequency), these findings possibly point to the importance of engaging participants with components of the psychological intervention. However, it is important to note that the studies included in the present systematic review and meta-analysis varied widely in the way in which engagement and the actual effectiveness of the stress-reducing psychological intervention was eval-uated.

The clinical relevance of the present findings is that we demonstrated that changes in immune parameters are found following the incorporation of a challenge into the design of the psychological intervention. Therefore, psychological interventions have the potential to alter the immune function which can be relevant to different disorders where immune function is affected. The current findings are in line with previous findings regarding the clinical relevance of altered immune function findings after psychological interventions.More specifically, a study of Antoni et al. [66] in women with breast cancer showed altered cytokine responses after a psychological intervention in response to an in vitroimmune-related challenge, suggesting that psychological interventions could optimize the host resistance to cancer. In another study of Antoni et al. [36], they found that a psychological intervention could buffer stress responses after a psychophysiological challenge (i.e., serostatus notification) in patients with HIV. Although the effect sizes in the context of in vitroimmune-related challenges were small in the present meta-analysis and no significant effects were found for psychophysiological challenges, in vivoimmune-related outcomes showed medium effect sizes. Therefore, the effects of psychological interventions might be less prominent at a cellular level, but it seems to be mainly expressed in response to chemical challenges. Particularly for the in vivochallenges, we found exploratory that the effectiveness of a psychological intervention in allergic participants by exposing them to skin tests yielded highest effect sizes [91-93]. These findings are presumably due to the fact that skin tests provide a rather sensitive immune function parameter in allergic patients. As the results of these studies provide insights not only in that a psychological intervention can alter immune function but also specifically in how a psychological intervention can result in less symptoms for an allergic condition, they aid in the understanding of the extent to which a psychological intervention can possibly support regular treatments for a specific condition, in this case allergic reactions. For this reason, it would be interesting to consider psychological interventions as add-on treatments to conventional medicine in allergic patients to buffer antiallergic responses. As this finding was based on exploratory analyses, future studies should investigate this further and preferably focus on incorporating a challenge that adequately matches the incorporated study sample. We also found a medium effect size for the effectiveness of psychological interventions in wound healing studies. Therefore, psychological interventions may be an effective add-on to conventional medicine, for example for surgery patients to facilitate recovery after a surgical intervention. To reduce the side effects of conventional medicine, it would also be interesting to investigate whether those interventions can at least partially replace regularly used treatments in, for example, patients with chronic ulcers.

The present meta-analytic review provides a rather comprehensive view on the effectiveness of psychological interventions on optimization of immune function by only incorporating studies that included a challenge into the study design, as more insights are gathered in the actual responsiveness of the immune system in response to a challenge. This not only contributes to the scientific literature but is also interesting for clinical practice. Furthermore, the present meta-analytic review extends the existing knowledge on the effectiveness of mind-body therapies in optimization of immune outcomes. More specifically, a descriptive review on the effectiveness of mind-body therapies in optimization of inflammatory markers already showed promising results [109]. However, mind-body therapies are based on multiple physical and psychological components. By including stress-reducing psychological interventions with cognitive behavior change techniques as the main component in the present meta-analytic review, more insights are gathered in the potential effectiveness of psychological intervention components in optimization of immune function. As the present meta-analytic review overall found a small positive effect of psychological interventions in optimization of immune function, with the highest effect sizes for studies incorporating in vivo immune-related challenges, future research should investigate whether psychological interventions can supplement, or possibly partially replace, current drug treatments in various somatic conditions to reduce side effects.

Besides the above-mentioned strengths, there are a couple of limitations that should be mentioned. First of all, due to the heterogeneity of the incorporated patient samples, psychological interventions, immune outcome parameters, and challenges of the included studies the present meta-analytic review could not provide a conclusive view on the effectiveness of psychological interventions on optimizing immune function. Future studies should systematically incorporate challenges to evaluate the effectiveness of a psychological intervention on immune function and adequately match the incorporated challenge(s) and psychological intervention with the included study population in order to gather a more homogeneous view on this topic. Second, we found additional studies based on screening of the reference lists of the included studies that were not identified in the primary search. Most of these studies did not specify immune outcome measures in the title and/or abstract. We cannot rule out that more studies were omitted in the present meta-analytic review due to a lack of sufficient specificity on the incorporation of immune outcome parameters in the title and/or abstract. It is important for future studies to specify immune outcome measures specifically in the title and/or abstract to ensure that they can be detected in searches. In addition, future systematic reviews and meta-analysis should be aware that some studies can be overlooked (i.e., in the case of studies that assess immune function without immune outcome parameters, as can be the case in wound healing studies) when including the immune outcome parameters as a category in their search. Therefore, a more general search term may be preferable when providing a systematic search on this topic. Third, a substantial number of studies did not report on whether the intervention was actually effective in reducing stress, making it hard to take this factor into account in our analyses. For the same reason, it was not possible to control for confounding factors, including BMI, recent illness, female menstruation cycle, and so on. As failures to improve immunity can be due to the fact that the stress-reducing psychological interventions were actually not effective in reducing stress, future studies should also carefully evaluate to what extent participants were engaged with the stress-reducing psychological intervention and whether these interventions were effective in reducing stress. To optimize the methodological aspects of the study design, future studies should take into account the recommendations of a recent review [112]. Finally, the present findings were based on the assumption that higher levels of immune activation were associated with a more optimized immune response. However, enhanced immune responses are not necessarily beneficial, e.g., in the case of inflammatory and autoimmune disorders, making it important to take the type of immune response into account [113]. In certain cases, optimization is not based on larger immune responses but rather on normalization of immune outcomes, making it hard to assess optimized immune function as a linear function and to compare various types of immune outcome parameters. Future studies should therefore take the incorporated immune parameters and the type of response, as well as the studied population, into account when evaluating the effectiveness of a psychological intervention on immune function. Note that, as the aim of a stress-reducing psychological intervention is to optimize health outcomes by stress reduction, it would be relevant to recruit individuals who experience chronic stress with a substantial impact on immune function to evaluate the effectiveness of stress-reducing psychological interventions [22]. In addition, future studies should focus on unraveling the effective intervention components in optimization of immune responses by evaluating the effectiveness of intervention components separately but also in combination with each other.

In conclusion, the present systematic review and meta-analysis provided evidence for the effects of stress-reducing interventions in optimization of immune function when immune outcomes were evaluated using tests that apply challenges to the immune system. While consistent evidence came from studies that evaluated immune function through an in vivo immune-related challenge, specifically studies incorporating skin tests and studies on wound healing, similar but smaller effect sizes were found for in vitro immune-related stimulations and immune responses to psychophysiological challenges. Due to the large heterogeneity in study designs, there is a need for future research that incorporates immune- and psychophysiological challenges, as these have a high external validity and are suitable for possible clinical applications in immune-related diseases. Studies in healthy participants have to make sure that the immune challenge is robust enough, e.g., by combining separate challenges. Finally, future studies should carefully report on the methodological details according to standardized guidelines, including the actual stress-reducing effectiveness of the psychological interventions, and appropriate interpretation of the immune outcomes. This can result in further insights into the immune outcomes that are responsive to change as well as a thorough view on the effective intervention components to optimize immune responses in the short and longer term.

The authors would like to thank Jan Schoones at the Library of Leiden University Medical Centre for his support with the search strategy.

The authors have no ethical conflicts to disclose.

The authors have no conflicts of interests to declare.

This work was supported by the European Research Council under the European Research Council Consolidator Grant (ERC-2013-CoG-617700_EXPECT HEAL-TH) and by the NWO under the NWO Vici Grant (016.Vici.170.152), both granted to A.W.M.E. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, the decision to publish, or the preparation of this paper. The authors declare that this research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interests.

Study conception and design: L.S., D.S.V., H.v.M., and A.W.M.E. Acquisition of data: L.S., D.S.V., P.I.C., and J.A.B. Analysis and interpretation of the data and writing of this paper: L.S., D.S.V., P.I.C., J.A.B., S.C., S.A.J., L.G.V, T.H.M.O., and A.W.M.E. All of the authors approved the submission of the final version of this paper.

1.
Horwitz
RI
,
Hayes-Conroy
A
,
Singer
BH
.
Biology, Social Environment, and Personalized Medicine
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2017
;
86
(
1
):
5
10
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
2.
Fava
GA
,
Cosci
F
,
Sonino
N
.
Current Psychosomatic Practice
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2017
;
86
(
1
):
13
30
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
3.
Tekampe
J
,
van Middendorp
H
,
Meeuwis
SH
,
van Leusden
JW
,
Pacheco-López
G
,
Hermus
AR
, et al.
Conditioning Immune and Endocrine Parameters in Humans: A Systematic Review
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2017
;
86
(
2
):
99
107
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
4.
Steptoe
A
,
Hamer
M
,
Chida
Y
.
The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating inflammatory factors in humans: a review and meta-analysis
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2007
Oct
;
21
(
7
):
901
12
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
5.
Segerstrom
SC
,
Miller
GE
.
Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry
.
Psychol Bull
.
2004
Jul
;
130
(
4
):
601
30
.
[PubMed]
0033-2909
6.
McCann
SM
.
Neuroimmunomodulation: Molecular aspects, integrative systems, and clinical advances
.
NY Acad Sci
;
1997
.
7.
Dhabhar
FS
.
Effects of stress on immune function: the good, the bad, and the beautiful
.
Immunol Res
.
2014
May
;
58
(
2-3
):
193
210
.
[PubMed]
0257-277X
8.
Walburn
J
,
Vedhara
K
,
Hankins
M
,
Rixon
L
,
Weinman
J
.
Psychological stress and wound healing in humans: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2009
Sep
;
67
(
3
):
253
71
.
[PubMed]
0022-3999
9.
Webster Marketon
JI
,
Glaser
R
.
Stress hormones and immune function
.
Cell Immunol
.
2008
Mar-Apr
;
252
(
1-2
):
16
26
.
[PubMed]
0008-8749
10.
Allen
AP
,
Kennedy
PJ
,
Cryan
JF
,
Dinan
TG
,
Clarke
G
.
Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: focus on the Trier Social Stress Test
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2014
Jan
;
38
:
94
124
.
[PubMed]
0149-7634
11.
Pedersen
AF
,
Zachariae
R
,
Bovbjerg
DH
.
Psychological stress and antibody response to influenza vaccination: a meta-analysis
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2009
May
;
23
(
4
):
427
33
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
12.
Pedersen
A
,
Zachariae
R
,
Bovbjerg
DH
.
Influence of psychological stress on upper respiratory infection—a meta-analysis of prospective studies
.
Psychosom Med
.
2010
Oct
;
72
(
8
):
823
32
.
[PubMed]
0033-3174
13.
Bekhbat
M
,
Neigh
GN
.
Sex differences in the neuro-immune consequences of stress: focus on depression and anxiety
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2018
Jan
;
67
:
1
12
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
14.
Cohen
S
,
Janicki-Deverts
D
,
Doyle
WJ
,
Miller
GE
,
Frank
E
,
Rabin
BS
, et al.
Chronic stress, glucocorticoid receptor resistance, inflammation, and disease risk
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2012
Apr
;
109
(
16
):
5995
9
.
[PubMed]
0027-8424
15.
Lewis
CE
,
O’Sullivan
C
,
Barraclough
J
.
The psychoimmunology of cancer: mind and body in the fight for survival?
USA
:
Oxford University Press
;
1994
.
16.
Butler
AC
,
Chapman
JE
,
Forman
EM
,
Beck
AT
.
The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses
.
Clin Psychol Rev
.
2006
Jan
;
26
(
1
):
17
31
.
[PubMed]
0272-7358
17.
Jain
S
,
Shapiro
SL
,
Swanick
S
,
Roesch
SC
,
Mills
PJ
,
Bell
I
, et al.
A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction
.
Ann Behav Med
.
2007
Feb
;
33
(
1
):
11
21
.
[PubMed]
0883-6612
18.
Househam
AM
,
Peterson
CT
,
Mills
PJ
,
Chopra
D
.
The effects of stress and meditation on the immune system, human microbiota, and epigenetics
.
Adv Mind Body Med
.
2017
;
31
(
4
):
10
25
.
[PubMed]
1532-1843
19.
Hulett
JM
,
Armer
JM
.
A systematic review of spiritually based interventions and psychoneuroimmunological outcomes in breast cancer survivorship
.
Integr Cancer Ther
.
2016
Dec
;
15
(
4
):
405
23
.
[PubMed]
1534-7354
20.
Black
DS
,
Slavich
GM
.
Mindfulness meditation and the immune system: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
.
2016
Jun
;
1373
(
1
):
13
24
.
[PubMed]
0077-8923
21.
Sharma
M
,
Rush
SE
.
Mindfulness-based stress reduction as a stress management intervention for healthy individuals: a systematic review
.
J Evid Based Complementary Altern Med
.
2014
Oct
;
19
(
4
):
271
86
.
[PubMed]
2156-5872
22.
Miller
GE
,
Cohen
S
.
Psychological interventions and the immune system: a meta-analytic review and critique
.
Health Psychol
.
2001
Jan
;
20
(
1
):
47
63
.
[PubMed]
0278-6133
23.
Vedhara
K
,
Fox
JD
,
Wang
EC
.
The measurement of stress-related immune dysfunction in psychoneuroimmunology
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
1999
May
;
23
(
5
):
699
715
.
[PubMed]
0149-7634
24.
Tekampe
J
,
van Middendorp
H
,
Sweep
FC
,
Roerink
SH
,
Hermus
AR
,
Evers
AW
.
Human Pharmacological Conditioning of the Immune and Endocrine System: challenges and Opportunities
.
Int Rev Neurobiol
.
2018
;
138
:
61
80
.
[PubMed]
0074-7742
25.
de Brouwer
SJ
,
Kraaimaat
FW
,
Sweep
FC
,
Creemers
MC
,
Radstake
TR
,
van Laarhoven
AI
, et al.
Experimental stress in inflammatory rheumatic diseases: a review of psychophysiological stress responses
.
Arthritis Res Ther
.
2010
;
12
(
3
):
R89
.
[PubMed]
1478-6354
26.
de Brouwer
SJ
,
van Middendorp
H
,
Kraaimaat
FW
,
Radstake
TR
,
Joosten
I
,
Donders
AR
, et al.
Immune responses to stress after stress management training in patients with rheumatoid arthritis
.
Arthritis Res Ther
.
2013
;
15
(
6
):
R200
.
[PubMed]
1478-6354
27.
Robinson
H
,
Norton
S
,
Jarrett
P
,
Broadbent
E
.
The effects of psychological interventions on wound healing: A systematic review of randomized trials
.
Br J Health Psychol
.
2017
Nov
;
22
(
4
):
805
35
.
[PubMed]
1359-107X
28.
Moher
D
,
Liberati
A
,
Tetzlaff
J
,
Altman
DG
;
PRISMA Group
.
Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement
.
Int J Surg
.
2010
;
8
(
5
):
336
41
.
[PubMed]
1743-9191
29.
Methley
AM
,
Campbell
S
,
Chew-Graham
C
,
McNally
R
,
Cheraghi-Sohi
S
.
PICO, PICOS and SPIDER: a comparison study of specificity and sensitivity in three search tools for qualitative systematic reviews
.
BMC Health Serv Res
.
2014
Nov
;
14
(
1
):
579
.
[PubMed]
1472-6963
30.
van Beugen
S
,
Ferwerda
M
,
Hoeve
D
,
Rovers
MM
,
Spillekom-van Koulil
S
,
van Middendorp
H
, et al.
Internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy for patients with chronic somatic conditions: a meta-analytic review
.
J Med Internet Res
.
2014
Mar
;
16
(
3
):
e88
.
[PubMed]
1438-8871
31.
Higgins
JP
,
Green
S
:
Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, version
,
2005
,
32.
Cohen
J
.
Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hilsdale
.
NJ
:
Lawrence Earlbaum Associates
;
1988
. p.
2
.
33.
Duval
S
,
Tweedie
R
.
Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis
.
Biometrics
.
2000
Jun
;
56
(
2
):
455
63
.
[PubMed]
0006-341X
34.
Solberg
EE
,
Halvorsen
R
,
Sundgot-Borgen
J
,
Ingjer
F
,
Holen
A
.
Meditation: a modulator of the immune response to physical stress? A brief report
.
Br J Sports Med
.
1995
Dec
;
29
(
4
):
255
7
.
[PubMed]
0306-3674
35.
McCain
NL
,
Gray
DP
,
Elswick
RK
,
Robins
JW
,
Tuck
I
,
Walter
JM
, et al.
A randomized clinical trial of alternative stress management interventions in persons with HIV infection
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
2008
Jun
;
76
(
3
):
431
41
.
[PubMed]
0022-006X
36.
Antoni
MH
,
Baggett
L
,
Ironson
G
,
LaPerriere
A
,
August
S
,
Klimas
N
, et al.
Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention buffers distress responses and immunologic changes following notification of HIV-1 seropositivity
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
1991
Dec
;
59
(
6
):
906
15
.
[PubMed]
0022-006X
37.
Christensen
AJ
,
Edwards
DL
,
Wiebe
JS
,
Benotsch
EG
,
McKelvey
L
,
Andrews
M
, et al.
Effect of verbal self-disclosure on natural killer cell activity: moderating influence of cynical hostility
.
Psychosom Med
.
1996
Mar-Apr
;
58
(
2
):
150
5
.
[PubMed]
0033-3174
38.
Davidson
RJ
,
Kabat-Zinn
J
,
Schumacher
J
,
Rosenkranz
M
,
Muller
D
,
Santorelli
SF
, et al.
Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation
.
Psychosom Med
.
2003
Jul-Aug
;
65
(
4
):
564
70
.
[PubMed]
0033-3174
39.
Goodin
BR
,
Quinn
NB
,
Kronfli
T
,
King
CD
,
Page
GG
,
Haythornthwaite
JA
, et al.
Experimental pain ratings and reactivity of cortisol and soluble tumor necrosis factor-α receptor II following a trial of hypnosis: results of a randomized controlled pilot study
.
Pain Med
.
2012
Jan
;
13
(
1
):
29
44
.
[PubMed]
1526-2375
40.
Green
ML
,
Green
RG
,
Santoro
W
.
Daily relaxation modifies serum and salivary immunoglobulins and psychophysiologic symptom severity
.
Biofeedback Self Regul
.
1988
Sep
;
13
(
3
):
187
99
.
[PubMed]
0363-3586
41.
Gruzelier
J
,
Smith
F
,
Nagy
A
,
Henderson
D
.
Cellular and humoral immunity, mood and exam stress: the influences of self-hypnosis and personality predictors
.
Int J Psychophysiol
.
2001
Aug
;
42
(
1
):
55
71
.
[PubMed]
0167-8760
42.
Gruzelier
J
,
Levy
J
,
Williams
J
,
Henderson
D
.
Self‐hypnosis and exam stress: comparing immune and relaxation‐related imagery for influences on immunity, health and mood
.
Contemp Hypn
.
2001
;
18
(
2
):
73
86
. 0960-5290
43.
Hayney
MS
,
Coe
CL
,
Muller
D
,
Obasi
CN
,
Backonja
U
,
Ewers
T
, et al.
Age and psychological influences on immune responses to trivalent inactivated influenza vaccine in the meditation or exercise for preventing acute respiratory infection (MEPARI) trial
.
Hum Vaccin Immunother
.
2014
;
10
(
1
):
83
91
.
[PubMed]
2164-5515
44.
Johnson
VC
,
Walker
LG
,
Heys
SD
,
Whiting
PH
,
Eremin
O
.
Can relaxation training and hypnotherapy modify the immune response to stress, and is hypnotizability relevant?
Contemp Hypn
.
1996
;
13
(
2
):
100
8
. 0960-5290
45.
Kaliman
P
,
Alvarez-López
MJ
,
Cosín-Tomás
M
,
Rosenkranz
MA
,
Lutz
A
,
Davidson
RJ
.
Rapid changes in histone deacetylases and inflammatory gene expression in expert meditators
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2014
Feb
;
40
:
96
107
.
[PubMed]
0306-4530
46.
Kiecolt-Glaser
JK
,
Glaser
R
,
Strain
EC
,
Stout
JC
,
Tarr
KL
,
Holliday
JE
, et al.
Modulation of cellular immunity in medical students
.
J Behav Med
.
1986
Feb
;
9
(
1
):
5
21
.
[PubMed]
0160-7715
47.
Kiecolt-Glaser
JK
,
Marucha
PT
,
Atkinson
C
,
Glaser
R
.
Hypnosis as a modulator of cellular immune dysregulation during acute stress
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
2001
Aug
;
69
(
4
):
674
82
.
[PubMed]
0022-006X
48.
Koh
KB
,
Lee
Y
,
Beyn
KM
,
Chu
SH
,
Kim
DM
.
Counter-stress effects of relaxation on proinflammatory and anti-inflammatory cytokines
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2008
Nov
;
22
(
8
):
1130
7
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
49.
Locke
SE
,
Ransil
BJ
,
Covino
NA
,
Toczydlowski
J
,
Lohse
CM
,
Dvorak
HF
, et al.
Failure of hypnotic suggestion to alter immune response to delayed-type hypersensitivity antigens
.
Ann N Y Acad Sci
.
1987
;
496
1 Neuroimmune I
:
745
9
.
[PubMed]
0077-8923
50.
McGrady
A
,
Conran
P
,
Dickey
D
,
Garman
D
,
Farris
E
,
Schumann-Brzezinski
C
.
The effects of biofeedback-assisted relaxation on cell-mediated immunity, cortisol, and white blood cell count in healthy adult subjects
.
J Behav Med
.
1992
Aug
;
15
(
4
):
343
54
.
[PubMed]
0160-7715
51.
Naito
A
,
Laidlaw
TM
,
Henderson
DC
,
Farahani
L
,
Dwivedi
P
,
Gruzelier
JH
.
The impact of self-hypnosis and Johrei on lymphocyte subpopulations at exam time: a controlled study
.
Brain Res Bull
.
2003
Dec
;
62
(
3
):
241
53
.
[PubMed]
0361-9230
52.
Pace
TW
,
Negi
LT
,
Adame
DD
,
Cole
SP
,
Sivilli
TI
,
Brown
TD
, et al.
Effect of compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune and behavioral responses to psychosocial stress
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
.
2009
Jan
;
34
(
1
):
87
98
.
[PubMed]
0306-4530
53.
Pennebaker
JW
,
Kiecolt-Glaser
JK
,
Glaser
R
.
Disclosure of traumas and immune function: health implications for psychotherapy
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
1988
Apr
;
56
(
2
):
239
45
.
[PubMed]
0022-006X
54.
Petrie
KJ
,
Booth
RJ
,
Pennebaker
JW
,
Davison
KP
,
Thomas
MG
.
Disclosure of trauma and immune response to a hepatitis B vaccination program
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
1995
Oct
;
63
(
5
):
787
92
.
[PubMed]
0022-006X
55.
Rosenkranz
MA
,
Davidson
RJ
,
Maccoon
DG
,
Sheridan
JF
,
Kalin
NH
,
Lutz
A
.
A comparison of mindfulness-based stress reduction and an active control in modulation of neurogenic inflammation
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2013
Jan
;
27
(
1
):
174
84
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
56.
Smith
GR
,
Conger
C
,
O’Rourke
DF
,
Steele
RW
,
Charlton
RK
,
Smith
SS
.
Psychological modulation of the delayed type hypersensitivity skin test
.
Psychosomatics
.
1992
;
33
(
4
):
444
51
.
[PubMed]
0033-3182
57.
Stetler
C
,
Chen
E
,
Miller
GE
.
Written disclosure of experiences with racial discrimination and antibody response to an influenza vaccine
.
Int J Behav Med
.
2006
;
13
(
1
):
60
8
.
[PubMed]
1070-5503
58.
Weinman
J
,
Ebrecht
M
,
Scott
S
,
Walburn
J
,
Dyson
M
.
Enhanced wound healing after emotional disclosure intervention
.
Br J Health Psychol
.
2008
Feb
;
13
(
Pt 1
):
95
102
.
[PubMed]
1359-107X
59.
Whitehouse
WG
,
Dinges
DF
,
Orne
EC
,
Keller
SE
,
Bates
BL
,
Bauer
NK
, et al.
Psychosocial and immune effects of self-hypnosis training for stress management throughout the first semester of medical school
.
Psychosom Med
.
1996
May-Jun
;
58
(
3
):
249
63
.
[PubMed]
0033-3174
60.
Zachariae
R
,
Hansen
JB
,
Andersen
M
,
Jinquan
T
,
Petersen
KS
,
Simonsen
C
, et al.
Changes in cellular immune function after immune specific guided imagery and relaxation in high and low hypnotizable healthy subjects
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
1994
;
61
(
1-2
):
74
92
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
61.
Zachariae
R
,
Kristensen
JS
,
Hokland
P
,
Ellegaard
J
,
Metze
E
,
Hokland
M
.
Effect of psychological intervention in the form of relaxation and guided imagery on cellular immune function in normal healthy subjects. An overview
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
1990
;
54
(
1
):
32
9
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
62.
Zachariae
R
,
Bjerring
P
,
Arendt-Nielsen
L
.
Modulation of type I immediate and type IV delayed immunoreactivity using direct suggestion and guided imagery during hypnosis
.
Allergy
.
1989
Nov
;
44
(
8
):
537
42
.
[PubMed]
0105-4538
63.
Andersen
BL
,
Farrar
WB
,
Golden-Kreutz
D
,
Emery
CF
,
Glaser
R
,
Crespin
T
, et al.
Distress reduction from a psychological intervention contributes to improved health for cancer patients
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2007
Oct
;
21
(
7
):
953
61
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
64.
Andersen
BL
,
Farrar
WB
,
Golden-Kreutz
DM
,
Glaser
R
,
Emery
CF
,
Crespin
TR
, et al.
Psychological, behavioral, and immune changes after a psychological intervention: a clinical trial
.
J Clin Oncol
.
2004
Sep
;
22
(
17
):
3570
80
.
[PubMed]
0732-183X
65.
Andersen
BL
,
Thornton
LM
,
Shapiro
CL
,
Farrar
WB
,
Mundy
BL
,
Yang
HC
, et al.
Biobehavioral, immune, and health benefits following recurrence for psychological intervention participants
.
Clin Cancer Res
.
2010
Jun
;
16
(
12
):
3270
8
.
[PubMed]
1078-0432
66.
Antoni
MH
,
Lechner
S
,
Diaz
A
,
Vargas
S
,
Holley
H
,
Phillips
K
, et al.
Cognitive behavioral stress management effects on psychosocial and physiological adaptation in women undergoing treatment for breast cancer
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2009
Jul
;
23
(
5
):
580
91
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
67.
Cohen
L
,
Parker
PA
,
Vence
L
,
Savary
C
,
Kentor
D
,
Pettaway
C
, et al.
Presurgical stress management improves postoperative immune function in men with prostate cancer undergoing radical prostatectomy
.
Psychosom Med
.
2011
Apr
;
73
(
3
):
218
25
.
[PubMed]
0033-3174
68.
Eremin
O
,
Walker
MB
,
Simpson
E
,
Heys
SD
,
Ah-See
AK
,
Hutcheon
AW
, et al.
Immuno-modulatory effects of relaxation training and guided imagery in women with locally advanced breast cancer undergoing multimodality therapy: a randomised controlled trial
.
Breast
.
2009
Feb
;
18
(
1
):
17
25
.
[PubMed]
0960-9776
69.
Fawzy
FI
,
Kemeny
ME
,
Fawzy
NW
,
Elashoff
R
,
Morton
D
,
Cousins
N
, et al.
A structured psychiatric intervention for cancer patients. II. Changes over time in immunological measures
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
1990
Aug
;
47
(
8
):
729
35
.
[PubMed]
0003-990X
70.
Gruber
BL
,
Hersh
SP
,
Hall
NR
,
Waletzky
LR
,
Kunz
JF
,
Carpenter
JK
, et al.
Immunological responses of breast cancer patients to behavioral interventions
.
Biofeedback Self Regul
.
1993
Mar
;
18
(
1
):
1
22
.
[PubMed]
0363-3586
71.
Larson
MR
,
Duberstein
PR
,
Talbot
NL
,
Caldwell
C
,
Moynihan
JA
.
A presurgical psychosocial intervention for breast cancer patients. psychological distress and the immune response
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2000
Feb
;
48
(
2
):
187
94
.
[PubMed]
0022-3999
72.
Lekander
M
,
Fürst
CJ
,
Rotstein
S
,
Hursti
TJ
,
Fredrikson
M
.
Immune effects of relaxation during chemotherapy for ovarian cancer
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
1997
;
66
(
4
):
185
91
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
73.
Lengacher
CA
,
Bennett
MP
,
Gonzalez
L
,
Gilvary
D
,
Cox
CE
,
Cantor
A
, et al.
Immune responses to guided imagery during breast cancer treatment
.
Biol Res Nurs
.
2008
Jan
;
9
(
3
):
205
14
.
[PubMed]
1099-8004
74.
Lengacher
CA
,
Kip
KE
,
Post-White
J
,
Fitzgerald
S
,
Newton
C
,
Barta
M
, et al.
Lymphocyte recovery after breast cancer treatment and mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) therapy
.
Biol Res Nurs
.
2013
Jan
;
15
(
1
):
37
47
.
[PubMed]
1099-8004
75.
McGregor
BA
,
Antoni
MH
,
Boyers
A
,
Alferi
SM
,
Blomberg
BB
,
Carver
CS
.
Cognitive-behavioral stress management increases benefit finding and immune function among women with early-stage breast cancer
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2004
Jan
;
56
(
1
):
1
8
.
[PubMed]
0022-3999
76.
Nelson
EL
,
Wenzel
LB
,
Osann
K
,
Dogan-Ates
A
,
Chantana
N
,
Reina-Patton
A
, et al.
Stress, immunity, and cervical cancer: biobehavioral outcomes of a randomized clinical trial [corrected]
.
Clin Cancer Res
.
2008
Apr
;
14
(
7
):
2111
8
.
[PubMed]
1078-0432
77.
Nunes
DF
,
Rodriguez
AL
,
da Silva Hoffmann
F
,
Luz
C
,
Braga Filho
AP
,
Muller
MC
, et al.
Relaxation and guided imagery program in patients with breast cancer undergoing radiotherapy is not associated with neuroimmunomodulatory effects
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2007
Dec
;
63
(
6
):
647
55
.
[PubMed]
0022-3999
78.
Richardson
MA
,
Post-White
J
,
Grimm
EA
,
Moye
LA
,
Singletary
SE
,
Justice
B
.
Coping, life attitudes, and immune responses to imagery and group support after breast cancer treatment
.
Altern Ther Health Med
.
1997
Sep
;
3
(
5
):
62
70
.
[PubMed]
1078-6791
79.
Savard
J
,
Simard
S
,
Ivers
H
,
Morin
CM
.
Randomized study on the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia secondary to breast cancer, part II: immunologic effects
.
J Clin Oncol
.
2005
Sep
;
23
(
25
):
6097
106
.
[PubMed]
0732-183X
80.
van der Pompe
G
,
Antoni
MH
,
Duivenvoorden
HJ
,
de Graeff
A
,
Simonis
RF
,
van der Vegt
SG
, et al.
An exploratory study into the effect of group psychotherapy on cardiovascular and immunoreactivity to acute stress in breast cancer patients
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2001
Nov-Dec
;
70
(
6
):
307
18
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
81.
van der Pompe
G
,
Duivenvoorden
HJ
,
Antoni
MH
,
Visser
A
,
Heijnen
CJ
.
Effectiveness of a short-term group psychotherapy program on endocrine and immune function in breast cancer patients: an exploratory study
.
J Psychosom Res
.
1997
May
;
42
(
5
):
453
66
.
[PubMed]
0022-3999
82.
Witek-Janusek
L
,
Albuquerque
K
,
Chroniak
KR
,
Chroniak
C
,
Durazo-Arvizu
R
,
Mathews
HL
.
Effect of mindfulness based stress reduction on immune function, quality of life and coping in women newly diagnosed with early stage breast cancer
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2008
Aug
;
22
(
6
):
969
81
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
83.
Coates
TJ
,
McKusick
L
,
Kuno
R
,
Stites
DP
.
Stress reduction training changed number of sexual partners but not immune function in men with HIV
.
Am J Public Health
.
1989
Jul
;
79
(
7
):
885
7
.
[PubMed]
0090-0036
84.
Esterling
BA
,
Antoni
MH
,
Schneiderman
N
,
Carver
CS
,
LaPerriere
A
,
Ironson
G
, et al.
Psychosocial modulation of antibody to Epstein-Barr viral capsid antigen and human herpesvirus type-6 in HIV-1-infected and at-risk gay men
.
Psychosom Med
.
1992
May-Jun
;
54
(
3
):
354
71
.
[PubMed]
0033-3174
85.
McCain
NL
,
Munjas
BA
,
Munro
CL
,
Elswick
RK
 Jr
,
Robins
JL
,
Ferreira-Gonzalez
A
, et al.
Effects of stress management on PNI-based outcomes in persons with HIV disease
.
Res Nurs Health
.
2003
Apr
;
26
(
2
):
102
17
.
[PubMed]
0160-6891
86.
Mulder
CL
,
Antoni
MH
,
Emmelkamp
PM
,
Veugelers
PJ
,
Sandfort
TG
,
van de Vijver
FA
, et al.
Psychosocial group intervention and the rate of decline of immunological parameters in asymptomatic HIV-infected homosexual men
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
1995
;
63
(
3-4
):
185
92
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
87.
Robinson
FP
,
Mathews
HL
,
Witek-Janusek
L
.
Psycho-endocrine-immune response to mindfulness-based stress reduction in individuals infected with the human immunodeficiency virus: a quasiexperimental study
.
J Altern Complement Med
.
2003
Oct
;
9
(
5
):
683
94
.
[PubMed]
1075-5535
88.
Germond
S
,
Schomer
HH
,
Meyers
OL
,
Weight
L
.
Pain management in rheumatoid arthritis: A cognitive-behavioural intervention
.
S Afr J Psychol
.
1993
;
23
(
1
):
1
9
. 0081-2463
89.
O’Leary
A
,
Shoor
S
,
Lorig
K
,
Holman
HR
.
A cognitive-behavioral treatment for rheumatoid arthritis
.
Health Psychol
.
1988
;
7
(
6
):
527
44
.
[PubMed]
0278-6133
90.
Zautra
AJ
,
Davis
MC
,
Reich
JW
,
Nicassario
P
,
Tennen
H
,
Finan
P
, et al.
Comparison of cognitive behavioral and mindfulness meditation interventions on adaptation to rheumatoid arthritis for patients with and without history of recurrent depression
.
J Consult Clin Psychol
.
2008
Jun
;
76
(
3
):
408
21
.
[PubMed]
0022-006X
91.
Kiecolt-Glaser
JK
,
Glaser
R
,
Williger
D
,
Stout
J
,
Messick
G
,
Sheppard
S
, et al.
Psychosocial enhancement of immunocompetence in a geriatric population
.
Health Psychol
.
1985
;
4
(
1
):
25
41
.
[PubMed]
0278-6133
92.
Koschwanez
HE
,
Kerse
N
,
Darragh
M
,
Jarrett
P
,
Booth
RJ
,
Broadbent
E
.
Expressive writing and wound healing in older adults: a randomized controlled trial
.
Psychosom Med
.
2013
Jul-Aug
;
75
(
6
):
581
90
.
[PubMed]
0033-3174
93.
Moynihan
JA
,
Chapman
BP
,
Klorman
R
,
Krasner
MS
,
Duberstein
PR
,
Brown
KW
, et al.
Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults: effects on executive function, frontal alpha asymmetry and immune function
.
Neuropsychobiology
.
2013
;
68
(
1
):
34
43
.
[PubMed]
0302-282X
94.
Vedhara
K
,
Bennett
PD
,
Clark
S
,
Lightman
SL
,
Shaw
S
,
Perks
P
, et al.
Enhancement of antibody responses to influenza vaccination in the elderly following a cognitive-behavioural stress management intervention
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2003
Sep-Oct
;
72
(
5
):
245
52
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
95.
Kern-Buell
CL
,
McGrady
AV
,
Conran
PB
,
Nelson
LA
.
Asthma severity, psychophysiological indicators of arousal, and immune function in asthma patients undergoing biofeedback-assisted relaxation
.
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback
.
2000
Jun
;
25
(
2
):
79
91
.
[PubMed]
1090-0586
96.
Witt
K
.
Psychological treatment can modulate the skin reaction to histamine in pollen allergic humans
.
Dermat Psychosom
.
2003
;
4
(
1
):
33
7
.
97.
Fry
L
,
Mason
AA
,
Pearson
RS
.
Effect of hypnosis on allergic skin responses in asthma and hay-fever
.
BMJ
.
1964
May
;
1
(
5391
):
1145
8
.
[PubMed]
0007-1447
98.
Beem
EE
,
Hooijkaas
H
,
Cleiren
MH
,
Schut
HA
,
Garssen
B
,
Croon
MA
, et al.
The immunological and psychological effects of bereavement: does grief counseling really make a difference? A pilot study
.
Psychiatry Res
.
1999
Jan
;
85
(
1
):
81
93
.
[PubMed]
0165-1781
99.
Bower
JE
,
Kemeny
ME
,
Taylor
SE
,
Fahey
JL
.
Finding positive meaning and its association with natural killer cell cytotoxicity among participants in a bereavement-related disclosure intervention
.
Ann Behav Med
.
2003
;
25
(
2
):
146
55
.
[PubMed]
0883-6612
100.
Elsenbruch
S
,
Langhorst
J
,
Popkirowa
K
,
Müller
T
,
Luedtke
R
,
Franken
U
, et al.
Effects of mind-body therapy on quality of life and neuroendocrine and cellular immune functions in patients with ulcerative colitis
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2005
;
74
(
5
):
277
87
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
101.
Mawdsley
JE
,
Jenkins
DG
,
Macey
MG
,
Langmead
L
,
Rampton
DS
.
The effect of hypnosis on systemic and rectal mucosal measures of inflammation in ulcerative colitis
.
Am J Gastroenterol
.
2008
Jun
;
103
(
6
):
1460
9
.
[PubMed]
0002-9270
102.
Doering
LV
,
Cross
R
,
Vredevoe
D
,
Martinez-Maza
O
,
Cowan
MJ
.
Infection, depression, and immunity in women after coronary artery bypass: a pilot study of cognitive behavioral therapy
.
Altern Ther Health Med
.
2007
May-Jun
;
13
(
3
):
18
21
.
[PubMed]
1078-6791
103.
Irwin
MR
,
Olmstead
R
,
Breen
EC
,
Witarama
T
,
Carrillo
C
,
Sadeghi
N
, et al.
Cognitive behavioral therapy and tai chi reverse cellular and genomic markers of inflammation in late-life insomnia: a randomized controlled trial
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2015
Nov
;
78
(
10
):
721
9
.
[PubMed]
0006-3223
104.
Hosaka
T
,
Matsubayashi
H
,
Sugiyama
Y
,
Izumi
S
,
Makino
T
.
Effect of psychiatric group intervention on natural-killer cell activity and pregnancy rate
.
Gen Hosp Psychiatry
.
2002
Sep-Oct
;
24
(
5
):
353
6
.
[PubMed]
0163-8343
105.
Arefnasab
Z
,
Babamahmoodi
A
,
Babamahmoodi
F
,
Noorbala
AA
,
Alipour
A
,
Panahi
Y
, et al.
Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction (MBSfR) and its effects on psychoimmunological factors of chemically pulmonary injured veterans
.
Iran J Allergy Asthma Immunol
.
2016
Dec
;
15
(
6
):
476
86
.
[PubMed]
1735-1502
106.
Broadbent
E
,
Kahokehr
A
,
Booth
RJ
,
Thomas
J
,
Windsor
JA
,
Buchanan
CM
, et al.
A brief relaxation intervention reduces stress and improves surgical wound healing response: a randomised trial
.
Brain Behav Immun
.
2012
Feb
;
26
(
2
):
212
7
.
[PubMed]
0889-1591
107.
Decety
J
,
Cacioppo
JT
.
The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience.
Oxford library of psychology,
2011
.
108.
Burns
VE
.
Using vaccinations to assess in vivo immune function in psychoneuroimmunology; PNEC
.
Springer
;
2012
. pp.
371
81
.
109.
Knudsen
TB
,
Daston
GP
;
Teratology Society
.
MIAME guidelines
.
Reprod Toxicol
.
2005
Jan-Feb
;
19
(
3
):
263
.
[PubMed]
0890-6238
110.
Janetzki
S
,
Britten
CM
,
Kalos
M
,
Levitsky
HI
,
Maecker
HT
,
Melief
CJ
, et al.
“MIATA”-minimal information about T cell assays
.
Immunity
.
2009
Oct
;
31
(
4
):
527
8
.
[PubMed]
1074-7613
111.
Schakel
L
,
Veldhuijzen
DS
,
van Middendorp
H
,
Prins
C
,
Joosten
SA
,
Ottenhoff
TH
, et al.
The effects of a psychological intervention directed at optimizing immune function: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial
.
Trials
.
2017
May
;
18
(
1
):
243
.
[PubMed]
1745-6215
112.
Guidi
J
,
Brakemeier
EL
,
Bockting
CL
,
Cosci
F
,
Cuijpers
P
,
Jarrett
RB
, et al.
Methodological Recommendations for Trials of Psychological Interventions
.
Psychother Psychosom
.
2018
;
87
(
5
):
276
84
.
[PubMed]
0033-3190
113.
Leclère
J
,
Weryha
G
.
Stress and auto-immune endocrine diseases
.
Horm Res
.
1989
;
31
(
1-2
):
90
3
.
[PubMed]
0301-0163
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes as well as any distribution of modified material requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.