Background: Genetic information, typically communicated in-person by genetic counselors, can be challenging to comprehend; delivery of this information online - as is becoming more common - has the potential of increasing these challenges. Methods: To address the impact of the mode of delivery of genomic risk information, 300 individuals were recruited from the general public and randomized to receive genomic risk information for type 2 diabetes mellitus in-person from a board-certified genetic counselor or online through the testing company's website. Results: Participants were asked to indicate their genomic risk and overall lifetime risk as reported on their test report as well as to interpret their genomic risk (increased, decreased, or same as population). For each question, 59% of participants correctly indicated their risk. Participants who received their results in-person were more likely than those who reviewed their results on-line to correctly interpret their genomic risk (72 vs. 47%, p = 0.0002) and report their actual genomic risk (69 vs. 49%, p = 0.002). Conclusions: The delivery of personal genomic risk through a trained health professional resulted in significantly higher comprehension. Therefore, if the online delivery of genomic test results is to become more widespread, further evaluation of this method of communication may be needed to ensure the effective presentation of results to promote comprehension.

The increase in the understanding of the impact of genetic variation for many different conditions and traits has resulted not only in the development of more genetic and genome-based tests, but also new routes of delivery. Specifically, direct-to-consumer testing enables consumers to order a range of services from single gene to genome-wide genotyping without the involvement of a health professional. The expanded use of testing both within medical specialties highly familiar with genetic and genomic testing (pediatrics and obstetrics/gynecology) and other less experienced medical specialties has also created a need to develop new approaches to counseling. Traditionally, genetic test results are communicated in-person, often by a genetic counselor. However, results may be returned online through the testing company or perhaps accessed through an electronic medical record, phone counseling and telemedicine.

The delivery of genetic test results through informal methods such as mail and phone has been demonstrated to be generally very reassuring with respect to anxiety, patient understanding and satisfaction [1,2,3,4], with some reported confusion and uncertainty about preventive interventions and familial risks [5]. With more than 70% of American adults estimated to have Internet access [6], the Internet presents a convenient mode for communicating test results, enabling individual control over the pace and amount of information consumed [7]. Interactive computer programs have been found to be more effective than standard genetic counseling for increasing knowledge of genetic testing [8,9]. Comfort with online communication about genetics has been associated with previous on-line health information - seeking a healthy life-style and positive attitudes towards genetics [10]. However, few studies have assessed the effectiveness of online communication of genetic or genomic risk with respect to understanding one's personal risk and decision making [11,12].

Several factors may influence an individual's ability to understand and emotionally process genomic risk information, including health literacy and genetic knowledge. Some evidence suggests that individuals with low health literacy may struggle to comprehend the information presented at a face-to-face genetic counseling session [13,14]. Specifically, lack of familiarity with genetic concepts and terms as well as preconceived perceptions of personal and familial risk may pose barriers to understanding genetic or genomic test results [5]. While strong factual knowledge of genetics seems likely to result in higher levels of comprehension of genomic risk, it is unclear whether this is an essential component for understanding risk and/or adopting healthy behavior to mitigate genomic risk [15]. Some studies have reported that women with lower health literacy recalled less information about a genetic test to predict breast cancer recurrence [16,17]; however, participants in these studies did not actually undergo genetic testing. The concept of being ‘at-risk' for healthy individuals may also prove challenging [18]. The ‘meaning' or implication of a test result is further framed by ethnicity and culture [19,20,21,22,23].

To address the effectiveness of in-person versus online delivery models for understanding genomic risk and adoption of healthy behaviors, we conducted a randomized clinical trial comparing delivery of genomic risk results for type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM). We also assessed the relationship between genetic knowledge and risk comprehension. We hypothesized that the delivery of genomic risk information in-person would result in greater comprehension. Furthermore, we hypothesized that the comprehension of genomic risk information would be higher in individuals with higher genetic knowledge, a positive family history, and/or college-level or higher education status. However, we also hypothesized that any differences between the 2 communication groups would be smaller in individuals with higher genetic literacy and education status. The analyses described here provide the foundation for future analysis and communication of genomic risk information to optimize public/patient understanding.

Overall Study Design

As reported elsewhere [24], we conducted a randomized study of 300 non-diabetic participants to explore the impact of (1) health literacy, genetic knowledge and attitudes about genomic testing, and (2) delivery approach of T2DM genomic risk on comprehension and perceptions of risk as well as changes in health. Participants were randomized to receive their results in-person from a certified genetic counselor or access them online through the testing laboratory's secure website. Participants were recruited from Duke University (Durham, N.C., USA) and surrounding areas. Participants completed a pre-testing survey to assess baseline knowledge and attitudes, family history and 3 follow-up surveys on risk comprehension and behavior change post-test, respectively. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Duke University Health System.

Testing

deCODE, Inc. conducted the T2DM genomic risk test for the study. Participants' gender and race were submitted with a buccal swab specimen for testing. As is deCODE's protocol, participants who self-identified as White were tested for SNPs in 4 genes: TCF7L2,PPARG,CDKAL1, and CDKN2A/B. Participants who self-identified as Asian were tested for SNPs in 3 genes: TCF7L2,CDKAL1, CDKN2A/B, and those who self-identified as African-American were tested for SNPs in the TCF7L2 gene. Participants who did not self-identify as one of these 3 categories were notified that results may not be as accurate or informative for them.

Delivery of Test Results

Test reports (online and print) included 2 types of risk information in varying formats: (1) relative genomic risk [numerically (e.g. 1.20)], text (e.g. 20% greater than someone of similar race and gender), and graphically [shaded bar: green (<1.0), yellow (= 1.0) and red (>1.0)]; and (2) overall lifetime risk estimate [numerically (percentage)], text (e.g. 20 out of 100), and pictorially [10 people figures with affected proportion shaded)]. Participants randomized to receive their test results online were provided a log-in name and password directly to the company's site; no changes were made to deCODE's site for the purposes of this study. Participants in the online arm could review their results as presented on the deCODE website (online suppl. fig. 1; for all online suppl. material, see www.karger.com/doi10.1159/000358413) or download a copy of their test report from the site (online suppl. fig. 2). Participants randomized to meet with the genetic counselor to review their test results were provided a print-out of the same test report accessible to participants in the online arm. Using the test report as a script, the genetic counselor reviewed each section of the report, highlighting each result, pointing to figures or section of the report to insure that that participant could follow along. The in-person review of results lasted approximately 10-15 min; additional time was necessary for some participants who had questions about the results or testing. Standard responses were developed to anticipate commonly asked questions. No additional information was provided without prompts from the participant to insure that participants in both study arms received the same information as much as possible.

Surveys

Participants were asked to complete a battery of surveys at baseline (pre-testing) to gather information on participant demographics and to assess health literacy, genetic knowledge and attitudes about genomic testing, and disease risk perception for T2DM, as described previously [24]. After receiving their test results, participants were surveyed at 1-week, 3-months and 6-months post-testing. The 1-week post-testing assessed their understanding of the test result, risk perception, psychosocial impact, test satisfaction, intention to change health behaviors, and information-seeking behaviors. To assess risk comprehension, we asked participants to interpret their genomic risk (increased risk, decreased risk or same as population risk - also referred to as categorical genomic risk in the paper). In addition, participants were asked to indicate (open-answer questions) their actual genomic risk score (reported as a relative risk on the test report) and overall lifetime risk (reported as a percentage). Since participants completed the online survey on their own, it is possible that some used their report to answer the questions about risk scores instead of relying on recall.

To assess perceptions and emotional response to health risks, we used the validated revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R) [25,26,27]. We assessed the psychosocial impact of testing using 3 subscales from the Multidimensional Impact of Cancer Risk Assessment (MICRA) questionnaire (the validated distress, uncertainty and relief from distress) [28]. Participants were also asked with whom they shared their test results and if they sought additional health information and from what sources (e.g. health professional, internet) [29]. To assess health literacy, we used the validated Short Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults (S-TOFHLA) [30,31]. We used a validated 16-item survey to measure actual knowledge about genetics (such as the association between genes, chromosomes, and cells and diseases) [32] and an 11-item survey to assess perceived knowledge of medical possibilities and social consequences of genetic testing [33].

Statistical Analysis

The impact of delivery methods on knowledge and interpretation of their genomic risk and associations to categorical participant characteristics was assessed using χ2 tests. These tests were used to analyze associations between categorical variables. Logistic regression was used to model the association between comprehension and the covariates of interest. Since only one participant was found to be of below adequate health literacy, this variable was not included in any further analysis. Change in agreement between patient responses to questions of risk was assessed using Kappa statistics and McNemar's test. Change in perceived risk after testing (1-week follow-up) was evaluated using Wilcoxon signed rank test and Spearman correlation coefficient. Two-sided p values are reported for all tests using a Type I error level of 0.05.

Participant Characteristics

Three hundred individuals were enrolled in the study; 257 (86%) and 239 (80%) participants completed the 1-week and 3-month follow-up survey, respectively (table 1). Enrolled participants were primarily female (70%), White (60%), between 18 and 29 years of age (44%), college-educated (65%) and reported a family history of T2DM (70%) [24]. Participants were randomized to receive their results in-person from a board-certified genetic counselor (n = 151) or to receive their results through the testing laboratory's secure website (n = 149).

Table 1

Characteristics of enrolled participants

Characteristics of enrolled participants
Characteristics of enrolled participants

Of the 257 participants who completed the 1-week follow-up survey, 115 (45%) participants were found to be at increased genomic risk, 118 (46%) at decreased, and 24 (9%) same as general population risk. The percentage of participants with increased genomic risk did not differ among those who did or did not report a family history of T2DM (p = 0.99).

Impact of Delivery Method on Comprehension

One week after receipt of their test results, we asked participants to indicate their genomic risk and overall lifetime risk as reported on their test report. In addition, participants were asked to interpret their genomic risk (increased, decreased or same as population). For each question, 59% of participants correctly indicated their risk (table 2). Thirty-eight percent of participants correctly indicated their genomic risk score and overall lifetime risk and correctly interpreted their genomic risk.

Table 2

Participant responses to questions about comprehension of their genomic and overall lifetime risk at the 1-week follow-up assessment (n=257)

Participant responses to questions about comprehension of their genomic and overall lifetime risk at the 1-week follow-up assessment (n=257)
Participant responses to questions about comprehension of their genomic and overall lifetime risk at the 1-week follow-up assessment (n=257)

At 1-week follow-up, we observed that a significantly greater proportion of participants who received the results in-person correctly interpreted their genomic risk (72%) compared to online (47%, p = 0.0002; table 3). However, the difference in correct reporting of overall lifetime risk between the study arms did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.154).

Table 3

Comparison of delivery models and level of correct interpretation and reporting of risk scores at the 1-week and 3-month follow-up

Comparison of delivery models and level of correct interpretation and reporting of risk scores at the 1-week and 3-month follow-up
Comparison of delivery models and level of correct interpretation and reporting of risk scores at the 1-week and 3-month follow-up

Participants who received their results in-person from the genetic counselor were again more likely to correctly interpret or report their genomic risk compared to those receiving their results online at 3 months, but, like the 1-week follow-up, no significant difference was observed between study arms with respect to correctly reporting overall lifetime risk score (table 3). We also analyzed retention-of-risk information between 1 week and 3 months and found that participants' likelihood to correctly report their genomic risk score and actual overall lifetime risk score decreased significantly (p < 0.001); no difference was observed with respect to retention of categorical genomic risk between 1 week and 3 months (p = 0.17) (data not shown). No significant difference was observed in the proportion of participants who accurately recalled either risk score or genomic risk interpretation between 1 week and 3 months between the study arms (in-person vs. online).

Impact of Genetic Knowledge and Other Factors on Comprehension of Test Results

As described previously, participants' knowledge of genetics ranged from 50 to 100% (mean = 84%; median = 88%) [24]. At 1-week follow-up, a one SD increase in genetic knowledge resulted in an estimated OR of comprehension (overall lifetime risk) of 1.3. (table 4).

Table 4

Logistic regression analysis of select participant factors and risk comprehension

Logistic regression analysis of select participant factors and risk comprehension
Logistic regression analysis of select participant factors and risk comprehension

Participants with a higher overall lifetime risk were observed to have a lower overall odds of correctly indicating their risk; using a logistic regression model, a 10-unit increase in overall risk results in a 0.73 decreased odds of correctly indicating overall lifetime risk (p = 0.02). Comprehension was significantly associated with the level of education for all risk comprehension questions (p < 0.001 for genomic risk score and categorical risk; p = 0.009 for overall lifetime risk). Risk comprehension (genomic or overall) and risk interpretation were not associated with age.

When comparing delivery methods to risk comprehension, the level of genetic knowledge did not impact comprehension. However, for participants with higher levels of educational status, their interpretation of risk and reporting of actual scores decreases in those who reviewed the results in-person with the genetic counselor (interaction p = 0.02 and 0.04, respectively).

Risk Perceptions

We asked participants to indicate their perceived risk of T2DM before and after receipt of their test results. At baseline, an equal proportion of participants (37% each) believed they were at either low or intermediate risk for developing T2DM in their lifetime, and 20% felt they were at high risk. Among those who perceived themselves at high risk, 27% reported a family history of T2DM in contrast to 4% reporting no family history (p < 0.001). No significant differences in perceived risk were observed at baseline with respect to race (White vs. non-White) or age. Women were more likely to perceive themselves at higher risk than men (p = 0.01) as well as participants with lower education level (p = 0.03).

At 1-week follow-up, a significant number of participants' perceived risk increased (p = 0.019). The change in risk assessment was correlated to the degree of actual risk (p = 0.0068), indicating improved concordance between perceived and actual genomic risk. No significant differences in perceived risk were observed at 1-week follow-up with respect to gender, education status or age. A significant association was observed between race and perceived risk (p = 0.015); further analysis revealed that this association is driven by differences in those who perceived themselves to be at intermediate or high risk. The direction of association differed between risk groups: participants who self-identified as White were more likely to indicate their risk as intermediate, and all other participants were more likely to perceive their T2DM risk as high. At 1-week follow-up, those with a family history of T2DM were no longer significantly more likely to indicate that they were at high risk (p > 0.05).

Perceived Causes and Illness Representations of T2DM

With respect to perceived causes of T2DM, participants were asked to consider 18 items from the IPQ-R causal beliefs subscale. At baseline, the largest proportion of respondents (57%) indicated that they strongly agreed that ‘diet or eating habits' was a factor affecting the risk of developing T2DM, followed by their ‘own behavior' (46%) and ‘heredity' (38%). When asked what they considered to be the primary risk factor, 47.1% indicated genes/family history, followed by diet (28%) and lifestyle (14%). More participants with family history of T2DM selected genes/family history as a risk factor (43%), than without (29%, p = 0.0432).

At 1-week follow-up, 44% of participants indicated that the primary risk factor for them was genes/family history, followed by diet (26%) and lifestyle (19%). There was not a significant difference in the proportion of participants at increased genomic risk who indicated genes/family history as the primary cause (p = 0.5144). In addition, no statistically significant difference in IPQ-R subscales and risk perception between those at increased and nonincreased genomic risk for T2DM or between those with and without a family history for other factors related to illness perception was observed.

Psychological Impact

At 1-week follow-up, the majority of participants expressed no regret after receiving their test results (92%). Based on the distress and uncertainty subscale from the MICRA, participants exhibited very low levels of distress (range: 0-30; median: 0; mean: 2.27). Distress subscale scores were not significantly different between participants with and without increased genomic risk for T2DM (p = 0.123), but were increased in participants with higher overall lifetime risk (Spearman rho = 0.33, p < 0.0001). Similarly, participants with and without increased genomic risk for T2DM did not show significant differences in uncertainty scores about their test results (range: 0-40; median: 3; p values = 0.22). The emotional representations subscale of the IPQ-R similarly demonstrated low levels of anxiety and depression based on T2DM results (range: 6-30; mean = 11.9). Of the 43% who had children, 54% indicated that they sometimes/often worried about the possibility of their children developing T2DM (regardless of their test result), and 17% indicated that they sometimes/often felt guilty about possibly passing on the risk of T2DM to their children.

Risk communication and comprehension are challenging issues for clinicians and patients alike, affected by several physician- and patient-related factors as well as patient risk perceptions [34]. For genetic and genomic testing, information about testing and test results are often delivered in-person by trained health professionals such as genetic counselors. Over the past decade, due to an increased number of genetic and genomic tests, limited access to counselors, and the launch of direct-to-consumer testing, genetic counseling services and the delivery of genetic and genomic risk information have evolved from in-person to phone-based counseling to self-access online. While evidence shows that discussing genetic testing with a counselor in-person [35,36,37] or over the phone [38,39] is effective in improving accuracy of perceived risk, the impact of online self-access to results on comprehension has not been ascertained. To our knowledge, we are the first group to assess the impact of delivery methods (in-person vs. online) of a genomic risk assessment on comprehension. Although our population was of high literacy (including numeracy), we found significant differences in comprehension associated with the mode of communication.

It may be reasonably anticipated that accessing one's genomic risk assessment without the opportunity to discuss it with a health professional can result in inaccurate comprehension. However, even with traditional modes of in-person genetic counseling, difficulties with genetic risk comprehension have been demonstrated for several conditions [11,36,40,41,42,43,44]. We found that participants receiving their T2DM genomic risk results in-person from a genetic counselor were significantly more likely to correctly indicate their risk compared to those receiving results online. About 69% of participants who received their results in-person from a genetic counselor were able to correctly indicate their genomic or lifetime risk score for T2DM and interpret their genomic risk, compared to 50% of participants receiving their results online. This finding aligns with reports that suggest genetic counseling (though limited to reporting of test results in this study) improves patients' accuracy of risk perception [45,46], but validates concerns raised about the potential for misinterpretation and confusion of results accessed online by consumers ordering from direct-to-consumer testing companies [47,48,49].

Irrespective of mode of communication or type of risk, about 59% of participants correctly comprehended their risk. Although there is limited data on comprehension of genomic risks delivered online, the level of risk comprehension of all participants in our study appears to fall below other published findings. In a population-based personalized medicine study with highly educated participants, 85% correctly reported their genomic risk (received online) for at least 1 of 7 diseases [50]. Similarly, another study with a comparable study population with respect to education reported that more than 80% of the participants correctly recalled their results for 8 common conditions within 10 days of reviewing their results (mailed to them) [51]. About half of participants acknowledged reviewing the whole report, and another 38% indicated they had read most of it [51]. Linnenbringer et al. [52] reported that 64% of participants with a family history of Alzheimer disease correctly indicated their genetic risk 6-weeks after receiving their test results in-person from a genetic counselor or physician. As we did not assess or inquire how much of the online report was reviewed, our low levels of comprehension may be partly due to the fact that they did not review the entire report (we could only confirm that they had logged on to access the report). Kaphingst et al. [53] tracked participants' access and time on web pages about genomic risk assessments and showed that participants' review of the websites varied by subject matter (tendency to review more introductory material than information about diseases or genes tested), averaging less than a third of the total number of pages available. Typically, participants reviewed the first few pages of a site; however, the first page of the site viewed by our participants included genomic risk and lifetime risk.

Comparison of our study with other reports may be difficult due to differences including participant characteristics, motivation to enrollment, types of disease risks reported, and familiarity with disease. In addition, several studies have shown differences in risk comprehension depending on the type of risk and how it is presented [54,55,56,57]. For example, risks can be presented as a possibility (categorical, descriptive, non-numeric statements) and/or probability (e.g. absolute or relative risk) [58] and in various other formats (numerically, descriptive, pictorially, graphically). In our study, the test report available to all participants included 2 types of risk information presented in multiple formats. Although the strategy of using multiple formats to present the same or related results may improve comprehension [54,55] (though not always [59]), we speculate that some participants (particularly those in the online arm) may have been overwhelmed with the amount of information presented in the test report, perhaps not recognizing that the figures and text conveyed the same information. As many genetic and genomic test results are reported as a probability, numeracy (the ability to use and understand numbers [55]) may be challenging [60,61,62,63,64,65]. If actual numerical risks are not needed or perceived to be of less value, perhaps the ‘less can be more' concept should be adopted to minimize confusion about the results [66,67]. Alternatively, test reports may be reorganized to present a simple one-page summary, perhaps containing possibility statements, followed by actual probabilities on subsequent pages to emphasize the take-home message.

The processing and comprehension of risk information can also be influenced by the manner in which it is received and the degree of participant involvement. Active involvement in disease risk communication portends greater likelihood for recall and behavior change as it involves ‘central processing' of the information [11,68]. In contrast, the receipt of test results online is a more passive form of communication and involves ‘peripheral processing,' which is more susceptible to counter persuasion [11]. Greater involvement in the process of risk communication is linked to the greater perceived relevance of the information to the individual.

We did not observe a higher likelihood of comprehension in participants at increased risk for T2DM, suggesting that the risk did not make a substantial impression or was not considered to be a cause for concern. This finding conflicts with Gordon et al. [50], who reported that more than half of participants who correctly reported their results specifically remembered diseases for which they were found to be at elevated risk. However, other studies have reported that normal results were more likely to be accurately recalled than mutation carriers [69].

Retention of risk scores decreased significantly by 3 months, but we found no significant change in correct interpretation of genomic risk at 3 months, comporting with other study findings of higher recall of categorical risk [50,70,71]. In contrast, Aktan-Collan et al. [72] reported near perfect recall one month and one year later for mutation status for hereditary non-polyposis colorectal cancer; however, individuals were uncertain what their actual risk was post-testing, particularly those who tested positive.The impact of learning of their genomic or overall lifetime risk in this population, already highly familiar with T2DM due to the high prevalence of family history, may account for the low retention rate. One study reported that T2DM was not perceived by some as a severe enough disease to warrant DNA-based testing (compared to the option of family history-based risk assessment) [73]. In contrast, of 8 conditions for which genomic risk (including 3 types of cancer) was provided to participants in another study, T2DM had the highest rate of correct risk interpretation [51].

Some evidence suggests that personalizing risk factors may increase individuals' perceived disease risk [74,75,76]. At baseline, our study population was divided between low and intermediate perceived risk, perhaps reflecting some optimistic bias given the high prevalence of reported family history [77,78,79]. Not surprisingly, those with family history were more likely to indicate a higher perceived risk than those without a family history, as reported elsewhere for T2DM and other diseases [78,80,81,82,83], though some exceptions have been reported [84]. Participants tended to readjust their perceived risks to the T2DM test result also comporting with other reported findings [85]. Given that heritability estimates of the currently known genomic biomarkers are about 10% compared to 40% for family history, the underappreciation of family history is of some concern [86,87,88,89].

Some limitations of our findings should be noted. The high educational status of participants may have positively influenced comprehension. Similarly, the high prevalence of family history of T2DM may also have biased responses, although positive family history was observed to be associated only with comprehension of overall risk. In addition to these factors, generalizability of the data may be limited due to self-enrollment from a limited geographic region. Given these limitations, further research is needed to fully assess the impact of delivery of genomic risk information on larger, more diverse populations as well as to compare levels of comprehension in response to different written/online presentation of risks.

In conclusion, while the opportunity to access test results directly without assistance from a trained health professional may offer benefits such as convenience and privacy, it may also adversely impact comprehension as patients attempt to understand their test results on their own. Thus, discussion of results with a health professional knowledgeable or familiar with genomic testing appears to be an important part of the testing process to increase the likelihood of risk comprehension and improved health outcomes. To accommodate the anticipated increase in genomic testing, however, changes may be needed to the delivery of genetic counseling services to enable greater access since in-person sessions may not be feasible, given the limited number and location of counselors. Alternative methods for patient education, counseling and risk communication will be needed to promote individuals' ability to understand genomic test results reviewed on their own.

This work was funded by the U.S. National Institutes of Health (1R21HL096573-01A1). This study is registered in clinicaltrials.gov as # NCT01186354.

1.
Sangha KK, Dircks A, Langlois S: Assessment of the effectiveness of genetic counseling by telephone compared to a clinic visit. J Genet Couns 2003;12:171-184.
2.
Gattas MR, MacMillan JC, Meinecke I, Loane M, Wootton R: Telemedicine and clinical genetics: establishing a successful service. J Telemed Telecare 2001;7(suppl 2):68-70.
3.
Coelho JJ, Arnold A, Nayler J, Tischkowitz M, MacKay J: An assessment of the efficacy of cancer genetic counselling using real-time videoconferencing technology (telemedicine) compared to face-to-face consultations. Eur J Cancer 2005;41:2257-2261.
4.
Peshkin BN, Schwartz MD, Isaacs C, Hughes C, Main D, Lerman C: Utilization of breast cancer screening in a clinically based sample of women after BRCA1/2 testing. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2002;11:1115-1118.
5.
Saukko PM, Ellard S, Richards SH, Shepherd MH, Campbell JL: Patients' understanding of genetic susceptibility testing in mainstream medicine: qualitative study on thrombophilia. BMC Health Serv Res 2007;7:82.
6.
Madden M: Internet evolution: Internet penetration and impact. Pew internet and American life project. Data memo. 2006. http://www.pewinternet.org/Reports/2006/Internet-Penetration-and-Impact.aspx.
7.
Bernhardt JM, Lariscy RA, Parrott RL, Silk KJ, Felter EM: Perceived barriers to internet-based health communication on human genetics. J Health Commum 2002;7:325-340.
8.
Green MJ, Biesecker BB, McInerney AM, Mauger D, Fost N: An interactive computer program can effectively educate patients about genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility. Am J Med Genet 2001;103:16-23.
9.
Green MJ, Peterson SK, Baker MW, Harper GR, Friedman LC, Rubinstein WS, Mauger DT: Effect of a computer-based decision aid on knowledge, perceptions, and intentions about genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2004;292:442-452.
10.
Bernhardt JM, McClain J, Parrott RL: Online health communication about human genetics: perceptions and preferences of internet users. Cyberpsychol Behav 2004;7:728-733.
11.
Emmons KM, Wong M, Puleo E, Weinstein N, Fletcher R, Colditz G: Tailored computer-based cancer risk communication: correcting colorectal cancer risk perception. J Health Commun 2004;9:127-141.
12.
Leighton JW, Valverde K, Bernhardt BA: The general public's understanding and perception of direct-to-consumer genetic test results. Public Health Genomics 2012;15:11-21.
13.
Roter DL, Erby L, Larson S, Ellington L: Oral literacy demand of prenatal genetic counseling dialogue: predictors of learning. Patient Educ Couns 2009;75:392-397.
14.
Roter DL, Erby LH, Larson S, Ellington L: Assessing oral literacy demand in genetic counseling dialogue: preliminary test of a conceptual framework. Soc Sci Med 2007;65:1442-1457.
15.
McBride CM, Koehly LM, Sanderson SC, Kaphingst KA: The behavioral response to personalized genetic information: will genetic risk profiles motivate individuals and families to choose more healthful behaviors? Annu Rev Public Health 2010;31:89-103.
16.
Brewer NT, Tzeng JP, Lillie SE, Edwards AS, Peppercorn JM, Rimer BK: Health literacy and cancer risk perception: implications for genomic risk communication. Med Decis Making 2009;29:157-166.
17.
Lillie SE, Brewer NT, O'Neill SC, Morrill EF, Dees EC, Carey LA, Rimer BK: Retention and use of breast cancer recurrence risk information from genomic tests: The role of health literacy. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2007;16:249-255.
18.
Scott S, Prior L, Wood F, Gray J: Repositioning the patient: the implications of being ‘at risk'. Soc Sci Med 2005;60:1869-1879.
19.
Catz DS, Green NS, Tobin JN, Lloyd-Puryear MA, Kyler P, Umemoto A, Cernoch J, Brown R, Wolman F: Attitudes about genetics in underserved, culturally diverse populations. Community Genet 2005;8:161-172.
20.
Bates BR, Lynch JA, Bevan JL, Condit CM: Warranted concerns, warranted outlooks: a focus group study of public understandings of genetic research. Soc Sci Med 2005;60:331-344.
21.
Brunk CG: Public knowledge, public trust: understanding the ‘knowledge deficit'. Community Genet 2006;9:178-183.
22.
Cunningham-Burley S: Public knowledge and public trust. Community Genet 2006;9:204-210.
23.
Bates BR: Public culture and public understanding of genetics: a focus group study. Public Underst Sci 2005;14:47-65.
24.
Haga SB, Barry WT, Mills R, Ginsburg GS, Svetkey L, Sullivan J, Willard HF: Public knowledge of and attitudes toward genetics and genetic testing. Genet Test Mol Biomarkers 2013;17:327-335.
25.
Marteau TM, Weinman J: Self-regulation and the behavioural response to DNA risk information: a theoretical analysis and framework for future research. Soc Sci Med 2006;62:1360-1368.
26.
Kaptein AA, van Korlaar IM, Cameron LD, Vossen CY, van der Meer FJ, Rosendaal FR: Using the common-sense model to predict risk perception and disease-related worry in individuals at increased risk for venous thrombosis. Health Psychol 2007;26:807-812.
27.
Moss-Morris R, Weinman J, Petrie KJ, Horne R, Cameron LD, Buick D: The revised illness perception questionnaire (ipq-r). Psychol Health 2002;17:1-16.
28.
Cella D, Hughes C, Peterman A, Chang CH, Peshkin BN, Schwartz MD, Wenzel L, Lemke A, Marcus AC, Lerman C: A brief assessment of concerns associated with genetic testing for cancer: the multidimensional impact of cancer risk assessment (micra) questionnaire. Health Psychol 2002;21:564-572.
29.
Arora NK, Hesse BW, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, Clayman ML, Croyle RT: Frustrated and confused: the American public rates its cancer-related information-seeking experiences. J Gen Intern Med 2008;23:223-228.
30.
Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nurss J: Development of a brief test to measure functional health literacy. Patient Educ Couns 1999;38:33-42.
31.
Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR: The test of functional health literacy in adults: a new instrument for measuring patients' literacy skills. J Gen Intern Med 1995;10:537-541.
32.
Jallinoja P, Aro AR: Knowledge about genes and heredity among Finns. N Genet Soc 1999;18:101-110.
33.
Morren M, Rijken M, Baanders AN, Bensing J: Perceived genetic knowledge, attitudes towards genetic testing, and the relationship between these among patients with a chronic disease. Patient Educ Couns 2007;65:197-204.
34.
Lautenbach DM, Christensen KD, Sparks JA, Green RC: Communicating genetic risk information for common disorders in the era of genomic medicine. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet 2013;14:491-513.
35.
Hopwood P, Shenton A, Lalloo F, Evans DG, Howell A: Risk perception and cancer worry: an exploratory study of the impact of genetic risk counselling in women with a family history of breast cancer. J Med Genet 2001;38:139.
36.
Meiser B, Halliday JL: What is the impact of genetic counselling in women at increased risk of developing hereditary breast cancer? A meta-analytic review. Soc Sci Med 2002;54:1463-1470.
37.
Wasson K, Sanders TN, Hogan NS, Cherny S, Helzlsouer KJ: Primary care patients' views and decisions about, experience of and reactions to direct-to-consumer genetic testing: a longitudinal study. J Community Genet 2013;4:495-505.
38.
Jenkins J, Calzone KA, Dimond E, Liewehr DJ, Steinberg SM, Jourkiv O, Klein P, Soballe PW, Prindiville SA, Kirsch IR: Randomized comparison of phone versus in-person BRCA1/2 predisposition genetic test result disclosure counseling. Genet Med 2007;9:487-495.
39.
Darst B, Madlensky L, Schork N, Topol E, Bloss C: Perceptions of genetic counseling services in direct-to-consumer personal genomic testing. Clin Genet 2013;84:335-339.
40.
Grimes DA, Snively GR: Patients' understanding of medical risks: implications for genetic counseling. Obstet Gynecol 1999;93:910-914.
41.
Lipkus IM, Klein WM: Effects of communicating social comparison information on risk perceptions for colorectal cancer. J Health Commun 2006;11:391-407.
42.
Martin W, Lobchuk M: Breast cancer risk perception and surveillance: an integrative review. Online J Knowl Synth Nurs 2003;10:2.
43.
Nagle C, Hodges R, Wolfe R, Wallace EM: Reporting Down syndrome screening results: women's understanding of risk. Prenat Diagn 2009;29:234-239.
44.
van Vliet HA, Grimes DA, Popkin B, Smith U: Lay persons' understanding of the risk of Down's syndrome in genetic counselling. BJOG 2001;108:649-650.
45.
Sivell S, Elwyn G, Gaff CL, Clarke AJ, Iredale R, Shaw C, Dundon J, Thornton H, Edwards A: How risk is perceived, constructed and interpreted by clients in clinical genetics, and the effects on decision making: systematic review. J Genet Couns 2008;17:30-63.
46.
Smerecnik CM, Mesters I, Verweij E, de Vries NK, de Vries H: A systematic review of the impact of genetic counseling on risk perception accuracy. J Genet Couns 2009;18:217-228.
47.
McGuire AL, Burke W: An unwelcome side effect of direct-to-consumer personal genome testing: raiding the medical commons. JAMA 2008;300:2669-2671.
48.
Ransohoff DF, Khoury MJ: Personal genomics: information can be harmful. Eur J Clin Invest 2010;40:64-68.
49.
Marietta C, McGuire AL: Currents in contemporary ethics. Direct-to-consumer genetic testing: is it the practice of medicine? J Law Med Ethics 2009;37:369-374.
50.
Gordon ES, Griffin G, Wawak L, Pang H, Gollust SE, Bernhardt BA: ‘It's not like judgment day': public understanding of and reactions to personalized genomic risk information. J Genet Couns 2012;21:423-432.
51.
Kaphingst KA, McBride CM, Wade C, Alford SH, Reid R, Larson E, Baxevanis AD, Brody LC: Patients' understanding of and responses to multiplex genetic susceptibility test results. Genet Med 2012;14:681-687.
52.
Linnenbringer E, Roberts JS, Hiraki S, Cupples LA, Green RC: ‘I know what you told me, but this is what I think:' Perceived risk of Alzheimer disease among individuals who accurately recall their genetics-based risk estimate. Genet Med 2010;12:219-227.
53.
Kaphingst KA, McBride CM, Wade C, Alford SH, Brody LC, Baxevanis AD: Consumers' use of web-based information and their decisions about multiplex genetic susceptibility testing. J Med Internet Res 2010;12:e41.
54.
Fagerlin A, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Ubel PA: Helping patients decide: ten steps to better risk communication. J Natl Cancer Inst 2011;103:1436-1443.
55.
Lipkus IM: Numeric, verbal, and visual formats of conveying health risks: suggested best practices and future recommendations. Med Decis Making 2007;27:696-713.
56.
Shepperd JA, Lipkus IM, Sanderson SC, McBride CM, O'Neill SC, Docherty S: Testing different communication formats on responses to imagined risk of having versus missing the GSTM1 gene. J Health Commun 2013;18:124-137.
57.
Malenka DJ, Baron JA, Johansen S, Wahrenberger JW, Ross JM: The framing effect of relative and absolute risk. J Gen Intern Med 1993;8:543-548.
58.
Zikmund-Fisher BJ: The right tool is what they need, not what we have: a taxonomy of appropriate levels of precision in patient risk communication. Med Care Res Rev 2013;70(suppl 1):37S-49S.
59.
Henneman L, Oosterwijk JC, van Asperen CJ, Menko FH, Ockhuysen-Vermey CF, Kostense PJ, Claassen L, Timmermans DR: The effectiveness of a graphical presentation in addition to a frequency format in the context of familial breast cancer risk communication: a multicenter controlled trial. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 2013;13:55.
60.
Abdel-Kader K, Dew MA, Bhatnagar M, Argyropoulos C, Karpov I, Switzer G, Unruh ML: Numeracy skills in ckd: Correlates and outcomes. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 2010;5:1566-1573.
61.
Lipkus IM, Samsa G, Rimer BK: General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples. Med Decis Making 2001;21:37-44.
62.
Rothman RL, Housam R, Weiss H, Davis D, Gregory R, Gebretsadik T, Shintani A, Elasy TA: Patient understanding of food labels: the role of literacy and numeracy. Am J Prev Med 2006;31:391-398.
63.
Schwartz LM, Woloshin S, Black WC, Welch HG: The role of numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography. Ann Intern Med 1997;127:966-972.
64.
Apter AJ, Paasche-Orlow MK, Remillard JT, Bennett IM, Ben-Joseph EP, Batista RM, Hyde J, Rudd RE: Numeracy and communication with patients: they are counting on us. J Gen Intern Med 2008;23:2117-2124.
65.
Lea DH, Kaphingst KA, Bowen D, Lipkus I, Hadley DW: Communicating genetic and genomic information: health literacy and numeracy considerations. Public Health Genomics 2011;14:279-289.
66.
Peters E, Dieckmann N, Dixon A, Hibbard JH, Mertz CK: Less is more in presenting quality information to consumers. Med Care Res Rev 2007;64:169-190.
67.
Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Fagerlin A, Ubel PA: A demonstration of ‘less can be more' in risk graphics. Med Decis Making 2010;30:661-671.
68.
Petty R, Cacioppo J: Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York, Springer, 1986.
69.
Harrison HF, Harrison BW, Walker AP, Lohman K, Ellis SD, Hall MA, Reiss J, Adams PC, Holup J, Acton RT, Bent T, Rivers C, Fadojutimi-Akinsiku M: Screening for hemochromatosis and iron overload: satisfaction with results notification and understanding of mailed results in unaffected participants of the HEIRS study. Genet Test 2008;12:491-500.
70.
Timmermans DR, Ockhuysen-Vermey CF, Henneman L: Presenting health risk information in different formats: the effect on participants' cognitive and emotional evaluation and decisions. Patient Educ Couns 2008;73:443-447.
71.
Vos J, Oosterwijk JC, Gómez-García E, Menko FH, Jansen AM, Stoel RD, van Asperen CJ, Tibben A, Stiggelbout AM: Perceiving cancer-risks and heredity-likelihood in genetic-counseling: how counselees recall and interpret BRCA1/2-test results. Clinical Genet 2011;79:207-218.
72.
Aktan-Collan K, Haukkala A, Mecklin JP, Uutela A, Kaariainen H: Comprehension of cancer risk one and 12 months after predictive genetic testing for hereditary non-polyposis colorectal cancer. J Med Genet 2001;38:787-792.
73.
Wijdenes-Pijl M, Dondorp WJ, Timmermans DR, Cornel MC, Henneman L: Lay perceptions of predictive testing for diabetes based on DNA test results versus family history assessment: a focus group study. BMC Public Health 2011;11:535.
74.
Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Warner TD: Effects of reviewing risk-relevant behavior on perceived vulnerability among women marines. Health Psychol 1991;10:173-179.
75.
Kreuter MW, Strecher VJ: Changing inaccurate perceptions of health risk: results from a randomized trial. Health Psychol 1995;14:56-63.
76.
Lipkus IM, Crawford Y, Fenn K, Biradavolu M, Binder RA, Marcus A, Mason M: Testing different formats for communicating colorectal cancer risk. J Health Commun 1999;4:311-324.
77.
Webster R, Heeley E: Perceptions of risk: understanding cardiovascular disease. Risk Manag Healthc Policy 2010;3:49-60.
78.
Claassen L, Henneman L, Kindt I, Marteau TM, Timmermans DR: Perceived risk and representations of cardiovascular disease and preventive behaviour in people diagnosed with familial hypercholesterolemia: a cross-sectional questionnaire study. J Health Psycol 2010;15:33-43.
79.
Acheson LS, Wang C, Zyzanski SJ, Lynn A, Ruffin MTt, Gramling R, Rubinstein WS, O'Neill SM, Nease DE Jr: Family history and perceptions about risk and prevention for chronic diseases in primary care: a report from the family healthware impact trial. Genet Med 2010;12:212-218.
80.
Hivert MF, Warner AS, Shrader P, Grant RW, Meigs JB: Diabetes risk perception and intention to adopt healthy lifestyles among primary care patients. Diabetes Care 2009;32:1820-1822.
81.
Buxton JA, Bottorff JL, Balneaves LG, Richardson C, McCullum M, Ratner PA, Hack T: Women's perceptions of breast cancer risk: are they accurate? Can J Public Health 2003;94:422-426.
82.
Caruso A, Vigna C, Marozzo B, Sega FM, Sperduti I, Cognetti F, Savarese A: Subjective versus objective risk in genetic counseling for hereditary breast and/or ovarian cancers. J Exp Clin Cancer Res 2009;28:157.
83.
Haber G, Ahmed NU, Pekovic V: Family history of cancer and its association with breast cancer risk perception and repeat mammography. Am J Public Health 2012;102:2322-2329.
84.
Pijl M, Henneman L, Claassen L, Detmar SB, Nijpels G, Timmermans DR: Family history of diabetes: exploring perceptions of people at risk in the Netherlands. Prev Chronic Dis 2009;6:A54.
85.
Ashida S, Koehly LM, Roberts JS, Chen CA, Hiraki S, Green RC: The role of disease perceptions and results sharing in psychological adaptation after genetic susceptibility testing: the REVEAL study. Eur J Hum Genet 2010;18:1296-1301.
86.
Herder C, Karakas M, Koenig W: Biomarkers for the prediction of type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2011;90:52-66.
87.
Meigs JB, Shrader P, Sullivan LM, McAteer JB, Fox CS, Dupuis J, Manning AK, Florez JC, Wilson PW, D'Agostino RB Sr, Cupples LA: Genotype score in addition to common risk factors for prediction of type 2 diabetes. N Eng J Med 2008;359:2208-2219.
88.
Voight BF, Scott LJ, Steinthorsdottir V, Morris AP, Dina C, et al: Twelve type 2 diabetes susceptibility loci identified through large-scale association analysis. Nat Genet 2010;42:579-589.
89.
Do CB, Hinds DA, Francke U, Eriksson N: Comparison of family history and SNPs for predicting risk of complex disease. PLoS Genet 2012;8:e1002973.
Copyright / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Copyright: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be translated into other languages, reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, microcopying, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.