Abstract
Background: A number of motor abnormalities have been reported in psychotic disorders, including dyskinesia and psychomotor slowing. There is also evidence for many of the same motor abnormalities in biological first-degree relatives and accruing evidence for motor abnormalities in bipolar disorder. In addition to motor dysfunction, there are also shared symptom domains amongst these populations. Objectives: We explored the associations of (1) current and lifetime psychosis and mood symptom domains and (2) domains of psychosis proneness with various domains of motor function in a transdiagnostic sample (n = 149). Method: Individuals with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, or bipolar disorder, biological first-degree relatives of individuals with a psychotic disorder, and controls completed measures of psychomotor speed and movement fluidity, and neural activity related to motor preparation (stimulus-locked lateralized readiness potential, S-LRP) and execution (response-locked LRP) was assessed using EEG. All participants completed the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; patients were additionally assessed for lifetime psychosis and mood episode symptoms, and relatives and controls completed the Chapman psychosis proneness scales. Results: Multiple regression revealed levels of current negative symptoms and mania were significantly positively associated with psychomotor slowing even after accounting for current antipsychotic medication dosage and duration of illness. S-LRP onset latency was significantly positively associated with magical ideation. Conclusion: Domains of motor function are associated with various mood and psychosis symptom domains in a transdiagnostic sample, which may provide insight into brain abnormalities relevant to the expression of symptoms across disorders.
Introduction
Empirical evidence of motor abnormalities in the psychosis spectrum has steadily accrued over the past few decades. The term “motor abnormalities” is intentionally broad, reflecting the diverse array of disturbances and dysfunction that have been reported in this population. These motor abnormalities range from involuntary movements (i.e., spontaneous dyskinesia [1, 2]), to disturbances in the execution of voluntary movement (e.g., disruptions in motor coordination and sequencing [3], slowness of movement [4]), and also include spontaneous Parkinsonism [1, 2] and the heterogeneous syndrome catatonia [5] (see Walther and Strik [6] and van Harten et al. [7] for reviews). It is important to note that all voluntary motor actions involve varying degrees of non-motor processes; therefore, “psychomotor abnormalities” is a more accurate descriptor for most of the motor abnormalities outlined above (see Morrens et al. [4] for discussion). The identification of motor abnormalities in the psychosis spectrum has highlighted specific brain regions that may be relevant to the pathophysiology of the disorder (e.g., cerebellar dysfunction [8]), and dyskinesia appears to have prognostic value for the development of a psychotic disorder [9, 10].
Motor function has also been studied in biological first-degree relatives of individuals with schizophrenia, who carry genetic liability for the disorder. Abnormalities in a number of domains of motor behavior have been reported in this population, suggesting that motor abnormalities may be an endophenotype reflecting genetic contributions to psychosis. For example, meta-analyses have indicated the presence of motor abnormalities in first-degree relatives that include spontaneous dyskinesia [2] (see also Koning et al. [11], but see Kent et al. [12]), spontaneous Parkinsonism [2] (see also Koning et al. [11] and Kamis et al. [13]), and neurological soft signs [14, 15]. Also, meta-analyses of cognitive and neuropsychological function have revealed psychomotor slowing in first-degree relatives [16, 17].
Outside of the psychomotor disturbances characteristic of mood states (i.e., psychomotor retardation and agitation; increased and decreased activity levels [18, 19]), relatively little attention has been paid to motor function in bipolar disorder. While there is less work examining motor function in bipolar disorder than first-degree relatives of individuals with schizophrenia, there are data to suggest that similar motor abnormalities exist in this population, although the extent to which each motor domain has been studied varies considerably. For example, a meta-analysis of euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder revealed psychomotor slowing in this population, though medication confounds complicated the interpretation of these results [20] (see also Correa-Ghisays et al. [21]). Neurological soft signs have been reported in bipolar disorder (see Hirjak et al. [22] and Peralta and Cuesta [23] for reviews), including in currently psychotic individuals [24, 25], individuals with no history of psychosis [26], and euthymic individuals [27]. Bolbecker et al. [28] reported balance impairments in euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder during a static task, while Kang et al. [29] reported impaired balance in depressed but not euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder during a dynamic task. Dyskinesia has also been reported in bipolar disorder [30, 31], including one study of euthymic individuals [32], as has Parkinsonism [30].
Findings of motor abnormalities in bipolar disorder are especially interesting when various similarities between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia are considered. Bipolar disorder and schizophrenia have been shown to share genetic risk factors [33]. In addition, there is substantial symptom overlap between psychotic and bipolar disorders. This is most apparent in schizoaffective disorder, wherein both mood and psychotic symptoms are prominent features, and the psychotic features that can be present during mood episodes in bipolar disorder. However, both mood symptoms and episodes also occur in schizophrenia [18], and the status of schizoaffective disorder as a distinct nosological entity has been called into question (see for example Peralta and Cuesta [34]). Given that symptom domains and motor abnormalities are shared by psychotic disorders and bipolar disorder, it would be informative to understand whether specific symptom domains are associated with particular motor abnormalities in a transdiagnostic sample. A relationship between a specific motor abnormality and transdiagnostic clinical phenotype could provide insight into the nature of the transdiagnostic occurrence of the motor dysfunction, and may indicate shared neural substrates, which could enhance our understanding of the underlying neural bases of various symptom domains.
A related question arises when considering subthreshold symptom expression in biological first-degree relatives. In addition to carrying genetic liability for psychosis, as a group these individuals display greater schizotypal symptoms than controls that are similar in content, but not in severity, to domains of symptomatology seen in psychosis (see for example Calkins et al. [35]). It is therefore of interest to examine if motor abnormalities in this population are associated with schizophrenia spectrum symptomology.
The current study aimed to investigate the mapping of different motor abnormalities onto dimensions of psychotic and mood symptomatology. As discussed above, there are many aspects of (psycho)motor functioning that have been investigated in the context of psychosis, and a comprehensive investigation including each type of motor abnormality that has been studied in psychosis is out of the scope of the current paper. In this study, we examined two aspects of motor behavior (movement fluidity and psychomotor speed) which we hypothesized to be related to specific symptom domains for associations with various psychotic, mood, and subthreshold psychosis symptom dimensions in transdiagnostic samples. We did the same in a more exploratory capacity for EEG measures of neural activity preceding action. Specifically, we examined the relationship of lifetime occurrence of mood and psychotic symptom domains to these motor variables in a sample of individuals with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, or bipolar disorder. We also examined the relationship of four domains of subthreshold psychotic symptomology (magical ideation, perceptual aberrations, social anhedonia, and physical anhedonia) to motor variables in controls and first-degree biological relatives of individuals with a psychotic disorder. Finally, the relationship of these motor variables to current levels of psychotic and mood symptom domains was explored across all participants.
It should be noted that unipolar depression is also associated with various psychomotor abnormalities (e.g., see Sobin and Sackeim [19] and Peralta and Cuesta [23] for reviews; see also Lohr et al. [36]). The associations between depression as a symptom domain and motor variables will be explored in the current study. However, given that the focus of this study is on the schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder/bipolar disorder spectrum, individuals with unipolar depression are not included as a patient group.
Our hypotheses regarding which symptom domains would be predictive of motor function were grounded in the literature specific to each motor domain investigated. There have been studies demonstrating the association of psychomotor slowing with negative symptoms in schizophrenia (see Morrens et al. [4] for review; see also Norman et al. [37], Docx et al. [38], and Bervoets et al. [39]). It has been hypothesized that psychomotor slowing is one motor component of a “negative syndrome” [38]. In addition, psychomotor slowing is a symptom of depression [18], and an association between depressive symptoms and psychomotor slowing has been reported in schizophrenia ([40]; see Morrens et al. [4] for review). The possibility that psychomotor slowing (and other deficits) could have the same neural and psychological substrates in individuals with psychotic disorders experiencing depression as those in individuals with depression has been suggested [40]. Therefore, we hypothesize that the negative symptoms and depression symptom domains will be associated with psychomotor slowing.
Dyskinesia results from basal ganglia abnormalities [41, 42], and striatal hyperdopaminergia is a possible contributing mechanism [42]. Striatal hyperdopaminergia has been hypothesized to be an important mechanism in the development of psychosis, particularly positive symptoms [43]. Given this possible shared mechanism, we hypothesize that only the positive symptoms of psychosis will be related to dysfluent movement. Importantly, the movement fluidity data reported here represent a secondary analysis of data previously published. In Kent et al. [12], we reported significantly increased movement dysfluency in a group of individuals with both schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder (compared to both controls and relatives), but no difference in movement fluency between controls and relatives. Given the possible shared mechanism discussed above, it is possible that increased dysfluency would only be expected in non-proband participants experiencing subthreshold “positive” symptoms. We therefore hypothesize that such symptoms (i.e., magical ideation and perceptual aberrations) will be associated with more dysfluent movement in the combined control and relative sample (note: these Chapman scales provide a richer characterization of these symptoms in comparison to the schizotypal personality questionnaire utilized in Kent et al. [12]).
Neural activity preceding action was investigated using the lateralized readiness potential (LRP). The LRP is calculated by subtracting EEG activity recorded from electrodes on opposite sides of the motor strip during a window preceding a response, with activity specific to the contralateral cortex believed to reflect processes related to motor preparation and execution [44, 45]. The LRP can be measured in relation to stimulus presentation or to response execution. The onset latency of the stimulus-locked LRP (S-LRP) reflects the time between stimulus presentation and the activation of motor preparatory processes, while the onset latency of the response-locked LRP (R-LRP) (measured in milliseconds before the response) reflects the time required for response execution once motor preparatory processes have been initiated [46]. Several studies have reported S-LRP onset latency delays in schizophrenia [46-49], while the literature examining R-LRP onset latency in schizophrenia has been mixed [46-50]. Here again, we are conducting a secondary analysis on data that we have previously published [51]. In Van Voorhis et al. [51], we reported significantly later S-LRP onset latency in both (1) individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and (2) individuals with bipolar disorder compared to first-degree relatives of individuals with a psychotic disorder, but not controls; there were no differences between groups in R-LRP onset latency. We do not have any specific hypotheses regarding which symptoms will be associated with variance in S-LRP onset latency, however given that relatives and controls were not significantly different from each other in Van Voorhis et al. [51], we hypothesize that subthreshold psychotic symptomology will be associated with S-LRP onset latency in these groups (i.e., any attenuated abnormality might only show up in individuals displaying some degree of subthreshold symptoms). Given the mixed findings regarding R-LRP onset latency in the literature, these analyses are considered exploratory.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The sample was composed of 149 individuals from the following groups: 32 individuals with schizophrenia (mean age = 40.31 years, SD = 11.44 years; 22 male), 14 individuals with schizoaffective disorder (10 with bipolar type, 4 with depressive type; mean age = 45.07 years, SD = 10.05 years; 9 male), 17 individuals with bipolar I disorder (mean age = 47.12 years, SD = 11.10 years; 14 male), 43 biological first-degree relatives of individuals with either schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (mean age = 45.86 years, SD = 10.96 years; 18 male), and 43 controls (mean age = 45.42 years, SD = 11.56 years; 24 male).
Three individuals in the schizophrenia group also had diagnoses of bipolar disorder not otherwise specified, and 9 individuals had other current or lifetime mood disorder diagnoses (n = 2 with major depressive disorder; n = 7 with depressive disorder not otherwise specified). Thirteen individuals with bipolar I disorder had a history of psychotic features during a mood state. Current mood states for the individuals with bipolar I disorder at the time of participation in this study were n = 4 depressed, n = 12 euthymic, and n = 1 unknown. All individuals in the patient groups were outpatients, and diagnoses were determined via clinical interview by trained psychodiagnosticians and subsequent review of these materials by doctoral students or doctoral-level staff (see Procedures section below for details).
One relative had a diagnosis of psychotic disorder not otherwise specified, one had a diagnosis of bipolar I disorder (current mood state euthymic, no history of psychotic features), and 12 had lifetime unipolar mood disorder diagnoses (n = 8 with major depressive disorder; n = 2 with depressive disorder not otherwise specified; n = 1 with a mood disorder due to a general medical condition; n = 1 with a substance-induced mood disorder). Two relatives (including one who had a unipolar mood disorder diagnosis) had Axis II Cluster A diagnoses (n = 1 with schizoid personality disorder; n = 1 with paranoid personality disorder).
Control participants had no current or past Axis I mood or psychotic disorders, no Axis II Cluster A personality disorders, and denied a family history of psychotic disorders. No participants had current alcohol or substance abuse or dependence diagnoses, and no participants in the patient groups were taking anti-Parkinsonian medications. In the patient groups, 54 individuals were taking antipsychotic medication (n = 5 taking typical antipsychotics); 33 individuals were taking mood stabilizers (n = 7 taking Lithium, n = 24 taking divalproex sodium, and n = 7 taking other mood stabilizers); 27 individuals were taking antidepressants (n = 10 taking SSRIs, n = 1 taking an MAOI, and n = 17 taking other antidepressants); and 8 individuals were taking benzodiazepines. Of the relatives carrying diagnoses described above, 6 were taking antidepressants, and 1 was taking an atypical antipsychotic medication. Chlorpromazine equivalent dosages were computed for individuals with complete antipsychotic medication dosage data [52].
Not all participants completed every motor task and clinical assessment. Table 1 provides the sample for each motor and clinical measure for each diagnostic group, as well as means and standard deviations. As discussed in the introduction, a portion of the data presented in this study is a secondary analysis of previously published data. Specifically, the movement fluidity data from the individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, relatives, and controls were presented in Kent et al. [12]. Movement fluidity data from the participants with bipolar I disorder have not been previously published. The participants with LRP data in this study are subsamples of the schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder, depressive type, bipolar disorder, relative, and control samples reported in Van Voorhis et al. [51], and reflect participants for whom both LRP and movement fluidity data were available. LRP data from individuals with schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type and an additional 6 relatives have not been previously published.
Procedures
Clinical and Cognitive Assessment
Procedures were approved by institutional review boards from the Minneapolis VA Health Care System and the University of Minnesota. Data collection occurred as a part of a larger study of individuals with a psychotic disorder or bipolar disorder and their biological first-degree relatives (see Docherty and Sponheim [53] for recruitment procedures). After an informed consent process, participants were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I [54]) and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS [55]). IQ was estimated using the Vocabulary and Block Design subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale [56]. Diagnoses (or lack thereof) were determined for each participant through review of all clinical research assessments by advanced clinical psychology graduate students or postdoctoral researchers in consultation with clinical psychologists, and a minimum of two such individuals reached consensus on all diagnoses.
The Operational Criteria Checklist for Psychotic and Affective Illness (OPCRIT) [57] was additionally completed for individuals with a psychotic or bipolar disorder. The OPCRIT records information on the lifetime presence and severity of various psychotic and mood episode symptoms. Controls and relatives of individuals with a psychotic disorder additionally completed the Chapman psychosis proneness scales measuring magical ideation [58], perceptual aberrations [59], physical anhedonia [60], and social anhedonia [61].
Assessments of Motor Function
To assess psychomotor speed, participants were administered the Trail Making Test Part A (Trails A) [62], with time (in seconds) serving as the dependent variable.
As stated in the introduction, this study represents a secondary analysis of the movement fluidity data presented in Kent et al. [12], wherein details regarding data collection and processing are outlined. Briefly, participants completed a handwriting task using a non-inking pen and a Wacom Intuos 3 digitizing tablet (Wacom, Saitama, Japan) connected to a computer. For the task, participants wrote “lleellee” in cursive in 1, 2, and 4 cm size conditions, completing five trials of each condition in random order. Each participant had a minimum of three acceptable trials (trials with too few strokes or that were interrupted were discarded), and the first eight strokes of each trial were subjected to movement fluidity analysis.
MovAlyzeR software (Neuroscript, LLC, Tempe, AZ, USA) was used to sample pen movements and for data processing and analysis. Data processing steps and additional analysis details are described in Caligiuri et al. [63] and Kent et al. [12]. Briefly, average normalized jerk (ANJ) was calculated [64] as an index of movement smoothness [65]. Jerk, the third time derivative, is calculated and normalized for vertical stroke size and duration (resulting in a unit-free value) [64] and averaged across strokes and trials. ANJ, which reflects changes in acceleration during movement, has been used previously to quantify dysfluent movement, or dyskinesia (indicated by higher ANJ values) [12, 63, 64, 66, 67]. Because our previous work demonstrated no main effect or interactions involving the size condition [12], we averaged ANJ across size conditions for the current secondary analysis.
Similarly, we are conducting a secondary analysis of LRP data for the current study. Data were collected as a part of a stop signal task (SST) paradigm, which was used to investigate the neural correlates of motor inhibition [51]. The SST comprised a Go Only condition and a Go/Stop condition. The current secondary analysis only examined data from the Go Only condition, wherein participants responded to an X replacing either the left or the right crosshair in a row of three crosshairs by pushing a button with their corresponding thumb as quickly as possible. In the Go/Stop condition, a stop signal appeared after a variable delay on 20% of trials, requiring participants to inhibit their responses. The SST had five blocks, each consisting of 20 Go Only trials, followed by 120 Go/Stop trials, and then 20 Go Only trials; there were never any stop trials during the Go Only trials.
Additional task details and full description of EEG data collection, processing, and analysis are provided in Van Voorhis et al. [51]. Briefly, EEG data were collected with either a 64- or 128-channel Ag/AgCl electrode array and a Biosemi ActiveTwo system (1,024 Hz sampling rate, single earlobe reference signal). Data were re-referenced to linked earlobes, filtered (0.5 Hz high-pass filter, 256 Hz low-pass filter), and de-noised via visual inspection and independent component analysis decomposition [68] and removal of noise ICs. Reconstituted de-noised data were average head re-referenced and epoched and averaged to form event-related potentials for various conditions [51].
The current analysis examines S-LRP and R-LRP onset latencies from the Go Only condition. As described in detail in Van Voorhis et al. [51], LRPs were calculated by low-pass filtering (8 Hz), baseline correcting, and then averaging waveforms for electrodes C3 and C4 for both left and right hand responses. Next, subtraction waveforms were computed to reflect LRP activity corresponding to both left and right hand responses and then averaged. See Van Voorhis et al. [51] for details regarding LRP onset latency calculations, as well as criteria for inclusion of LRP data for each participant.
Statistical Analysis
As an index of current psychotic and mood symptom severity, we averaged BPRS item ratings corresponding to each of the five factors identified by Wilson and Sponheim [69] (positive symptoms, negative symptoms, disorganization, mania symptoms, and depression/anxiety symptoms). As an index of lifetime psychotic and mood symptom expression, we multiplied binary (i.e., symptom coded as present or not present) OPCRIT items by their respective factor loadings [70], which were then summed according to the five factors identified by Dikeos et al. [70] (mania, reality distortion, depression, disorganization, and negative symptoms). Total number of pathological items endorsed was summed for each of the four Chapman scales.
Multiple regression models were used to investigate the association between the domains of each of the three symptom measures (BPRS, OPCRIT, and Chapman Scales) and each of the motor indices (Trails A, ANJ, and LRP). There was a significant pairwise correlation observed between R-LRP and S-LRP onset latency (r = 0.42, p < 0.001), prompting the use of multivariate multiple regression with two dependent variables (S-LRP onset latency, R-LRP onset latency) for all models predicting LRP onset latency. In all models, age and sex were included as covariates due to their observed association with the outcome variables (age associated with S-LRP (p < 0.001) and Trails A (p = 0.019); sex associated with ANJ (p = 0.001) and S-LRP (p = 0.005)). Number of errors was included as a covariate for all models predicting Trails A completion time. Each model included only participants who completed both of the relevant measures; the number of participants from each group included in each model is listed in Table 2.
For models involving the patient groups that yielded significant effects, we conducted follow-up analyses to explore and account for possible effects of antipsychotic medication and duration of illness on the variables of interest. Specifically, models were re-run including only patients for whom complete antipsychotic medication dosage data were available, and then these new models were run again including chlorpromazine equivalent dosages and duration of illness as covariates.
Results
The model investigating the relationship between current symptom severity, measured using the BPRS, and Trails A performance yielded several significant associations (Fig. 1). Time to complete Trails A was significantly associated with positive symptoms (β = 0.22, t = 2.09, p = 0.040), mania (β = 0.20, t = 2.02, p = 0.047), and negative symptoms (β = 0.24, t = 2.33, p = 0.022) (full model statistics: F(8, 96) = 4.44, p = 0.0001, adjusted R2 = 0.21). All associations were in the positive direction, meaning that increased symptoms were associated with increased time to complete Trails A. There was no significant association between BPRS factor severity and ANJ (all p values >0.1). There was no significant association between BPRS factor severity and either of the LRP indices (all p values >0.1).
Amongst the lifetime symptom factors in patients, measured using the OPCRIT, there was a trend-level association between reality distortion (delusions and hallucinations) and Trails A completion time (β = 0.45, t = 1.87, p = 0.081) (full model statistics: F(8, 15) = 1.14, p = 0.3952, adjusted R2 = 0.05). The direction of this association indicates that greater lifetime reality distortion symptoms were associated with longer Trails A completion time. However, this result stems from a markedly smaller sample than the previously reported BPRS findings (n = 24, vs. n = 105 in the BPRS analysis). Reality distortion showed a significant association with S-LRP onset latency (β = –0.37, t= –2.36, p = 0.026) (full model statistics for S-LRP: F(7, 27) = 3.27, p = 0.012, adjusted R2 = 0.32). The negative association in this reality distortion finding suggests that these symptoms were related to faster S-LRP onset latency when accounting for all other OPCRIT symptom factors; however, it is important to note that the sample for this analysis was also quite small (n = 35). There was no significant association between OPCRIT factors and the other motor indices (all p values >0.1).
Subthreshold symptom severity, measured using the Chapman scales, was not associated with Trails A or ANJ (all p values >0.1). However, there was a significant association between the magical ideation score and S-LRP onset latency (β = 0.42, t = 2.99, p = 0.005) (full model statistics for S-LRP: F(6, 41) = 4.22, p = 0.002, adjusted R2 = 0.29) (Fig. 2). The positive direction of this association indicates that greater magical ideation was associated with longer S-LRP onset latency. This analysis did not include the two relatives with psychotic disorder not otherwise specified and bipolar disorder diagnoses due to lack of Chapman data from these participants. β, t, and p values for all models are listed in Table 3.
Associations between the Chapman scales and S-LRP onset latency (* p < 0.01).
Follow-Up Analyses
When the BPRS-Trails A model was re-run including only those participants for whom complete antipsychotic medication dosage data were available (n = 102, 3 fewer participants), the significant associations between Trails A and negative symptoms (β = 0.28, t = 2.54, p = 0.013) and mania (β = 0.24, t= 2.28, p = 0.025) persisted, and the association between Trails A and positive symptoms dropped to β = 0.17, t = 1.63, p = 0.106 (full model statistics: F(8, 93) = 3.69, p = 0.0009, adjusted R2 = 0.18). When chlorpromazine equivalent dosage and duration of illness were entered into the model, both negative symptoms (β = 0.30, t = 2.71, p = 0.008) and mania (β = 0.21, t = 2.02, p = 0.046) were still significantly associated with Trails A (positive symptom association p = 0.419). Chlorpromazine equivalent dosage was not significantly associated with Trails A (p = 0.858), and there was a trend-level association wherein longer duration of illness was associated with longer Trails A time (β = 0.22, t = 1.73, p = 0.087) (full model statistics: F(10, 91) = 3.35, p = 0.0009, adjusted R2 = 0.19).
When the OPCRIT-S-LRP model was re-run including only those participants for whom complete antipsychotic medication dosage data were available (n = 30, 5 fewer participants), the significant association between reality distortion and S-LRP onset latency persisted (β = –0.42, t = –2.53, p = 0.019) (full model statistics: F(7, 22) = 3.19, p = 0.017, adjusted R2 = 0.35). When chlorpromazine equivalent dosage and duration of illness were entered into the model, the significant association between reality distortion and S-LRP onset latency persisted (β = –0.42, t = –2.44, p = 0.024). Neither chlorpromazine equivalent dosage (p = 0.615) nor duration of illness (p = 0.623) were significantly associated with S-LRP onset latency (full model statistics: F(9, 20) = 2.37, p = 0.0516, adjusted R2 = 0.30).
In order to further examine the counterintuitive positive association between mania and Trails A, we conducted an exploratory analysis wherein status regarding taking divalproex sodium, a mood stabilizer with possible parkinsonian motor side effects [71], was included as a binary (i.e., taking or not taking divalproex sodium) covariate in the model already including chlorpromazine equivalent dosage and duration of illness as covariates. In this model, negative symptoms (β = 0.26, t = 2.30, p = 0.024) were still significantly associated with Trails A, and the association between mania and Trails A dropped to the trend level (β = 0.18, t = 1.79, p = 0.076). Neither chlorpromazine equivalent dosage (p = 0.844) nor duration of illness (p = 0.422) were significantly associated with Trails A in this model, but there was a trend-level association wherein taking divalproex sodium was associated with longer Trails A time (β = 0.22, t = 1.86, p = 0.066) (full model statistics: F(11, 90) = 3.45, p = 0.0005, adjusted R2 = 0.21).
Discussion
Our examination of the association of deviations in motor function with symptomatology across psychotic and bipolar disorders revealed that multiple domains of current symptom severity are associated with psychomotor slowing. When current antipsychotic medication dosage and duration of illness were included in the model, the relationships of negative symptoms and mania with psychomotor slowing are robust. While the association between negative symptoms and slower psychomotor activity is consistent with hypotheses and previous research (see Introduction), the finding of increased mania being associated with slower psychomotor activity is unexpected given the psychomotor features characteristic of mania. While exploratory post hoc medication analysis suggested that this relationship could be partially accounted for by mood stabilizing medication (though see below in the discussion for important caveats), the persistence of this relationship at the trend level is notable. Like all psychomotor tasks [4], Trails A involves non-motor processes. While speculative, it is possible that the association between Trails A completion time and mania severity reflects the impact of mania symptoms on the non-motor processes recruited (e.g., attention, goal directedness) by this task. It is important to note that variability in completion time associated with Trails A errors was accounted for in the models.
The finding of an association between magical ideation and S-LRP onset latency was broadly consistent with our hypothesis, indicating that S-LRP onset latencies more similar to those seen in psychosis are associated with more subthreshold psychosis symptoms. This finding compliments existing literature showing delayed S-LRP onset latency in individuals with psychosis [46-49], and suggests that S-LRP onset latency abnormalities in non-psychotic individuals are associated with intermediate symptom expression rather than genetic liability per se [51]. Longer S-LRP onset latency is reflective of delayed response selection and initiation of response preparation. However, to better understand what this abnormality associated with schizophrenia spectrum symptoms reflects, these findings must be considered in the context of the task. In a similar task, Kappenman et al. [46] demonstrated that task-relevant (i.e., responding as quickly and accurately as possible) higher-order processes affecting response selection and motor preparation explained S-LRP onset delays in individuals with schizophrenia. In addition, fMRI studies of speeded responding have implicated the basal ganglia, pre-SMA (supplementary motor area), and frontal areas [72-74].
It is difficult to know how to interpret the relationship between lifetime reality distortion symptoms and S-LRP onset latency in patients, as this relationship is in the opposite direction as that in controls and relatives. The smaller sample size for this analysis may be an important factor, and future work examining the relationship of motor function to indices of lifetime symptoms should include larger samples.
There are several limitations of the current study that must be addressed. First, antipsychotic medications have known motor side effects, and the fact that the majority of the patients in this study were taking antipsychotic medication poses a major confound for the study of motor function. We have attempted to account for effects related to current antipsychotic medication dosage and approximate duration of antipsychotic treatment by re-running all models with significant effects with chlorpromazine equivalent dosages and duration of illness included. However, motor side effects of antipsychotic medication are certainly related to more than simply current dosage, and while we have attempted to account for duration of treatment with antipsychotics by including duration of illness as a covariate as well, this is a very rough proxy and of course reflects additional factors. Furthermore, there is also the issue of shared variance between such covariates and diagnosis/symptoms, with this also being a serious concern for the exploratory analysis including on versus off divalproex sodium, making it difficult to draw firm conclusions regarding variance accounted for by medication. In summary, we have attempted to understand the effects that some medications may be having on our dependent variables, but this study is not designed to interrogate these questions.
In addition to the possibility of motor side effects, psychotropic medications impact symptoms as well, and therefore future research should study the relationship between symptom domains and motor function in medication-naïve samples. An additional limitation of this study relates to sample size. Because not all measures were administered to all participants, some models had smaller sample sizes than others, with the model examining the relationship of lifetime symptomology to psychomotor slowing in patients particularly underpowered, as previously noted.
A final limitation concerns the scope of the current study and of the particular motor measures used. Indeed, we have only been able to begin interrogating the broader question of the relationships between transdiagnostic motor dysfunction and clinical phenomenology. This is reflected in both the fact that we only included measures of three aspects of motor function, and in the limitations inherent to the specific measures we used. For example, Trails A assesses both motor speed and visual search [75], and it is therefore possible that the relationship between Trails A performance and symptoms reflects the relationship between visual search ability and symptoms to some degree. Importantly, while visual search ability could be considered a confound in the context of the current study given the nature of the Trails A task, it is essential to remember that all psychomotor functioning involves a variety of motor (e.g., speed, coordination) and non-motor processes (e.g., visuospatial monitoring, higher-order processes) [4]. Attempts to separate motor and non-motor processes are futile and given that non-motor processes are indeed essential to motor behavior [4], any such attempts, even if possible, would be of limited utility. In addition, as we have previously discussed [12], handwriting fluency is a less demanding neuromuscular control task than force stability maintenance; it is therefore possible that a different measure of dyskinesia could have been more sensitive to more subtle levels of dyskinesia. Finally, as discussed above, task parameters have been shown to affect group differences in S-LRP onset latency [46], thereby circumscribing the generalizability of our findings.
In conclusion, the results of the present study demonstrate the relationship between psychotic and mood symptom domains and domains of motor dysfunction in a transdiagnostic sample. Both mania and negative symptoms were associated with psychomotor slowing. In addition, a relationship between variability in psychomotor processes and subthreshold symptomology was demonstrated for S-LRP onset latency, which was related to magical ideation. It is possible that the pattern regarding which symptom domains are associated with various motor indices may highlight particular neural substrates as being potentially relevant for the expression of certain symptoms. Furthermore, investigation of the relationship of transdiagnostic symptom domains with motor function may aid in further identifying motor abnormalities relevant to psychopathology, which dovetails with the recent addition of a sensorimotor domain to the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria framework.
Acknowledgement
We are grateful to Seung Suk Kang and Mallory Skorheim for assistance with EEG and handwriting data analysis, respectively.
Statement of Ethics
All participants gave written informed consent, and the study protocol was approved by the Minneapolis VA Health Care System and the University of Minnesota IRBs. This research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Disclosure Statement
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Funding Sources
This research was supported by a Merit Review Award #I01CX000227 from the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs, Clinical Sciences Research and Development Research Program, and award number U01MH108150 from the National Institute of Mental Health to S.R.S. J.S.K. was supported by a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (award number F32MH112334) from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the National Institutes of Health. These funding sources played no role in preparation of data or the manuscript.
Author Contributions
J.S.K., S.G.D., and S.R.S. conceived of the study. S.R.S. oversaw data collection. S.G.D., J.S.K., A.C.V.V., S.U., M.P.C., and S.R.S. analyzed the data. J.S.K. and S.G.D. drafted the manuscript, S.R.S. provided critical feedback and substantially edited the manuscript, and M.P.C. and S.U. provided critical feedback on the manuscript.