Effortful control (EC) or self-regulation refers to the ability to regulate behavior, emotion, and cognition. It has been identified as a contributor to both adaptive and adverse outcomes in children, adolescents, and adults and this across many domains. As such, it could be considered as a transdiagnostic dimension underlying internalizing (e.g., mood and anxiety disorders) and externalizing (e.g., substance use disorders, ADHD) psychopathology. We aimed to examine the role of EC throughout the adult psychopathological spectrum by means of a literature search of studies published between 2008 and 2018. Overall, the results point to the role of EC in the development of a broad spectrum of psychiatric diagnosis, reflecting the transdiagnostic characteristic of this construct. This role may be both directly causal or as a mediator factor influencing outcomes of a specific disorder. Early assessment of EC and early interventions to improve EC might help to avoid or decrease the risk of developing psychopathology.

The Research Domain Criteria (RDoc) framework [1, 2] grew out of a need to develop new ways of classifying mental disorders for research purposes, based on dimensions of observable behavior and measurable neurobiology. As such, the RDoC framework promotes taking underlying neurobiological factors into account when exploring their role in the pathogenesis of different phenotypical psychopathological presentations. One of the RDoC constructs that has received large attention is that of “control” within the cognitive RDoC domain. Control refers to a system that modulates the operation of other cognitive and emotional systems, in the service of goal-directed behavior, when prepotent modes of responding are not adequate to meet the demands of the current context. This connects closely with the concept of self-regulation (SR) which is considered to have an important role in adaptive and adverse outcomes in children, adolescents, and adults. In temperament research, the construct of “effortful control” is used for studying this SR in children and adolescents whereas in clinical neurocognitive research one uses the construct of “executive functioning” [3, 4].

In the psychobiological model of Rothbart (Fig. 1), temperament is defined as “constitutionally based individual differences in reactivity and SR, influenced over time by heredity, maturation and experience” [5]. Reactivity (e.g., punishment sensitivity related to negative affectivity, reward sensitivity related to positive affectivity, bottom-up regulation) includes responsiveness of emotional, activation, and arousal systems. In Gray’s Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory [6, 7], reactive dimensions of temperament are described in levels of behavioral approach system (BAS) and levels of behavioral inhibition system (BIS), whereby the BAS is sensitive for reward and the BIS is sensitive to punishment [8]. Higher BIS sensitivity is reflected in higher proneness to anxiety and is related to the personality trait of Neuroticism in the Five Factor Model (FFM) [9], elevated BAS sensitivity is reflected in elevated proneness to engage in goal directed efforts and to experience positive feelings and is related to the personality trait of Extraversion in the FFM [8-12]. Reactive temperament can be assessed using the Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System Scales (BIS/BAS) [8].

Fig. 1.

Figure illustrating the model of Rothbart [5].

Fig. 1.

Figure illustrating the model of Rothbart [5].

Close modal

SR or effortful control (EC) involves the behaviors and processes that one uses to modulate one’s reactivity or reactions to stimuli [13, 14]. Whereas reactivity can be observed early in childhood, SR is developed later in life and progresses from simple reactive control to a more flexible, intentional, and self-directed use of attentional processes often referred to as EC [15]. This process parallels the maturation of the prefrontal cortex and may be conceptualized as top-down regulation of behavior. EC moderates the influence from BIS and BAS. As an aspect of temperament, EC allows flexible regulation of thought, emotion, and is therefore strongly related to executive functioning [16-18].

EC is defined as “the efficiency of executive attention – including the ability to inhibit a dominant response and/or to activate a subdominant response, to plan, and to detect errors” [13 p. 129]. The EC concept consists of 3 components: (1) attentional control which involves the ability to voluntarily focus or shift attention, (2) inhibitory control which is the ability to effortfully inhibit behavior, and (3) activation control which involves the ability to activate behavior even when one is not fully motivated [13, 14, 19]. Regulative temperament can be measured using the Effortful Control Subscale of the Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ-EC) [20]. In sum, it can be stated that EC is the ability to approach a situation which one rather avoids and to inhibit actions which one desires [18] and can be seen as the fundamental basis of SR [15].

EC as a broad concept has been proven to be an important factor with respect to both normal, successful human development and the vulnerability to psychopathology [17] and thus covering a continuum from normality/normal range of personality to psychopathology. With respect to the pathogenesis of mental disorders, poor EC is associated with both internalizing and externalizing psychopathology, i.e. depression, bipolar disorder, eating disorders (ED), personality disorders (PD), ADHD, and addiction [17, 21-26]. Clinically, individuals with low levels of EC are at increased risk for multiple types of psychopathological symptoms across disease categories suggesting, EC can be seen as a transdiagnostic construct.

It is self-evident that impulse control-related diseases (ADHD, substance abuse, ED, behavior addictions) are characterized by disturbances of the inhibitory control component of the EC definition, but the attentional control and activation control components of the EC definition are also relevant aspects of EC in, e.g., the affective disorders.

Although it would be interesting to distinguish between the three components of EC in both internalizing and externalizing disorders, in most studies however the total sum of EC is used since the general concept of EC is of interest [27] and the alpha coefficients of the subscales are often too low, indicating that they are less reliable than the total score [28].

The neurobiological substrate of the EC network has been proposed to constitute of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the lateral prefrontal cortex [29-31]. This so-called executive attention network functions via top-down processes. The ACC is involved in the detection and monitoring of conflict, whereas lateral prefrontal areas have been shown to be mainly related to conflict resolution [32, 33]. In addition to the role of the ACC in the regulation of cognitive processing, this area appears to be also involved in the regulation of emotions [34] since activation in the dorsal part of the ACC has been seen when participants were performing a Stroop-like task, but in ventral areas when the task involved emotional stimuli [35].

The theoretical link between EC and the executive attention network has resulted in several studies investigating the link between EC self-reports (questionnaires) and performance on executive function tasks [36-38]. Concerning the self-report measures, EC is often measured using temperament questionnaires (self-report questionnaires, e.g. the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire [EATQ] [39], and the Effortful Control Scale [ECS]) [40]. Another measure often used in studies with adults is the conscientiousness scale of the Big Five NEO-FFI [41]. Within the domain of the performance-based tasks, Fan et al.[42] developed the Attention Network Task (ANT) to assess the different attentional networks (and their interrelations) within a single task. More specific, the efficiency of alerting, orienting, and executive attention is measured by evaluating the impact of alerting cues, spatial cues, and flankers on reaction time. Another battery that is used for assessing EC in young children is the Kochanska’s Battery for Assessing Effortful Control (Kochanska’s EC battery). It has tasks that assess abilities for delaying or waiting, slowing down of gross and fine motor activity, suppressing or activating behavior, effortful attention, and lowering of voice or whispering [43]. Inhibitory control is the component of EC that is most studied and which often uses parent or teacher questionnaires for assessing children’s EC or for adults a self-report questionnaire (e.g., ECS) and behavioral tasks for measurement. Behavioral measures/neuropsychological mea-sures often used to tap inhibitory control are the Go/No-Go task (requires responding to one frequent occurring stimulus and inhibiting a response to another less frequently occurring stimulus), the Stroop task (requires ignoring a dominant perceptual feature of a stimulus in favor of a subdominant feature, e.g. naming the color in which a word is printed, instead of reading the word), and the Stop-Go task (requires inhibition of an about-to-be-executed (prepared) motor response [44, 45] besides tasks to tap attentional control (i.e., attention focusing and attention control) [for an overview, see 18]. Another task to study conflict resolution is the flanker task in which a target stimulus is surrounded by irrelevant stimulation that can either match or conflict with the response required by the target [46].

Concerning the questionnaires, we found the Adult Temperament Questionnaire the most reliable and valid as it is known to have a good internal consistency and convergent validity with other measures of temperament and personality. The scale is divided into three subscales: inhibitory control, attentional control, and activation control. Internal consistency for the full scale in the original version was α = 0.78 (inhibitory control α = 0.60, attentional control α = 0.73, and activation control α = 0.69) [28]. Normative data for EC have been reported in a large non-clinical sample (n = 700) [20].

Regarding the performance-based tests, the following measurements have proven the most reliable and valid indicators of EC: Stroop Color and Word test [47], the Go/No-Go Task and the Stop Signal Task [45].

Questionnaires and behavioral measures of inhibition in general show small to moderate correlations, and results seem to depend on the type of neuropsychological measure that is used to tap executive functions. Another explanation of the low correspondence of no correspondence could be that temperament questionnaires measure rather more general identity-related beliefs about experience, whereas neuropsychological tasks assess the momentary situation-related behavioral performance [23, 48, 49]. There is some evidence for a positive relationship between EC and executive attention, but especially when parent reports of the EC of their child are considered. Some research studies therefore question the interchangeability of EC questionnaires and neuropsychological executive functioning tasks when measuring EC [50].

Taken together, EC is considered an important construct within the RDoC cognitive system domain, and cross-sectional studies show its involvement in a broad spectrum of traditional-categorical (DSM-ICD) psychiatric diagnoses. From this perspective, EC might be considered as a prototypical transdiagnostic dimension. However, no recent review has been published exploring the role throughout the adult psychopathological spectrum. Within the current work, we aimed at exploring its role in both internalizing and externalizing psychiatric disorders.

Our narrative review was based upon a literature search in the database PubMed for studies published with search terms EC and adults and clinical disorders (internalizing and externalizing disorders, mood disorders, depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, ED, impulsive disorders, ADHD, substance use disorder (SUD), and behavioral disorders (i.e., gambling, internet gaming, compulsive internet use [CIU], and compulsive buying [CB]). Limits were set in time, between 2008 and 2018, adult age (>18 years) and English. We only included original research papers that reported well-accepted self-report questionnaires or behavioral measures for EC. The combination of the aforementioned search items led to the identification of 29 articles (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Flow chart.

For each study, relevant data, extracted by the first author, addressed the characteristics and findings of the selected studies. Findings are reported for each pathology/psychiatric diagnosis separately, and only significant findings are reported (Table 1).

Table 1.

Study characteristics, outcome measures, and main findings

Study characteristics, outcome measures, and main findings
Study characteristics, outcome measures, and main findings

In total, 29 unique original research articles were included in this review.

EC and Mood and Anxiety Disorders

Reactive (i.e., punishment sensitivity/negative affectivity, reward sensitivity/positive affectivity) and regulative (EC and attentional mechanisms) temperament factors are increasingly being seen as possible mechanisms underlying increased vulnerability to depression and anxiety [9, 51, 52].

The study of Dinovo and Vasey [53] provides evidence for extending a temperamental model for depression and anxiety to adults. They also stress the importance of taking the regulatory component of temperament, namely EC, into account in understanding the association between temperamental activity and depressive and anxious disorders. They found that persons high in negative reactivity, low in positive reactivity, and low in EC were especially vulnerable for symptoms of depression. Overall, the authors showed an important role of EC in internalizing (depressive, anxious) symptoms, i.e. both as a main effect and as a moderator of the effect of reactive aspects of temperament (BIS/BAS). Of note, this was a (large: n = 477) study in undergraduate students (as part of their academic curriculum), not an identified patient sample, exploring symptoms and not disorders. High levels of negative affect and low levels of EC were also associated with higher levels of anxiety in a study by Tortella-Feliu et al. [54], whereby the prospective association between negative affectivity and avoidance was moderated by EC (high negative affect being related to engaging in avoidance behaviors only for those exhibiting low EC). In a longitudinal study in university students, Nishiguchi et al. [55] explored the temporal relationship between EC, depressive symptoms, and cognitive motivation (need for cognition [NfC]). NfC is a personality trait related to one’s motivation to enjoy effortful cognitive activity. Their findings suggest a temporal relation whereby depressive symptoms caused a decrease in EC both as a main effect and indirectly, mediated by cognitive motivation. This finding could imply that a motivational deficit may partially explain the decreased EC found in depressed patients. Their results moreover showed that the indirect effect of depressive symptoms was significant for all EC subscales. Since inhibition control, activation control and attentional control all require some cognitive effort, the decline of NfC associated with depressive symptoms may predict less efficiency of all aspects of EC.

Clements and Bailey [56] studied the relationship between anxiety and aspects of temperament in their effort to develop a profile that may be used to identify individuals at risk. In their large sample (509 undergraduate students), high positive affect and high EC correlated with low levels of reported anxiety. Another study performed in non-clinical young adults examined the relationship between negative emotionality and depressive symptoms through mediation or moderation of cognitive vulnerability factors (cognitive style, brooding, stress-reactive rumination) and EC but found no moderating role of EC between negative emotionality and cognitive vulnerability factors [57].

A study of Kanske and Kotz [58] showed that EC as a temperamental trait and subclinical depression and anxiety together influence the emotional modulation of executive attention. Participants high in EC and low in depression and anxiety, especially, responded faster to conflict processing (flanker test, Simon task) in emotional stimuli, showed an enhanced ERP conflict negativity, and additional (fMRI) increased activation in the ventral ACC. Of note, this was a study in healthy controls (HC) where subclinical depressive and anxious symptoms were explored, not a clinical population with distinct mood or anxiety disorders. Marchetti et al. [59] compared EC and attentional networks in remitted depressed patients (RMD) and HC as measured with the Adult Temperament Questionnaire-Effortful Control (ATQ-EC) and the ANT. In their sample of 90 RMD and 270 HC, high EC was substantially associated with less depressive and anxiety symptoms regardless of clinical status (i.e., RMD vs. HC) while less efficient executive attention was modestly associated with more depressive complaints in the whole sample. Of interest, no differences were found in EC between HC and RMD. The negative correlation between depressive symptoms and EC could represent an attentional dysfunction, as attentional bias especially for negative information [60] and diminished attentional control are associated with depression.

EC and Bipolar Disorder

Burdick et al. [61] performed a study on 24 patients with bipolar I or bipolar II disorder (both in the depressive phase), and an HC group with performance-based tasks in order to measure attention and psychomotor functioning. Relative to the HC, the bipolar sample demonstrated evidence of psychomotor slowing and revealed deficits on measures of effortful attention, yet demonstrated comparable performance on measures of automatic attention. These results suggest an attentional impairment during the depressed phase of bipolar disorder that may be specific to effortful processing, while automatic processes remain relatively intact. Murphy et al. [64] found manic patients were impaired in their ability to inhibit behavioral responses (low levels of inhibitory control, subcomponent of EC) and focus attention, but depressed patients were impaired in their ability to shift the focus of attention and exhibited an affective bias for negative stimuli (low levels of attentional control, subcomponent of EC).

EC and ED

Aspects of reactive and regulative temperament also play an important role in the etiology of ED. EC is one of the constructs most often named in research investigating the development and maintenance of ED. Several studies found individuals with high NA and low EC are more at risk for developing ED symptomatology [63-65]. Low EC was also found to be associated with impulsiveness in binging/purging ED [63, 66].

As mentioned above, several studies showed that high NA and low EC place individuals at risk for developing ED symptomatology.

Zwaan et al. [67] investigated the association between reactive and regulative aspects of temperament and severe weight cycling in overweight and obese individuals. They found that higher reward sensitivity/BAS was associated with more severe weight cycling, but no association or moderating effect with EC was found. In a study concerning the role of temperamental factors in binge eating in obese patients, patients with regular binge eating reported a significantly lower level of EC compared with patients without binge eating [63]. Interestingly, the binge eating group did not show lower BIS and/or higher BAS scores (reactive temperament), both related to more impulsivity. The study of Turner et al. [68] identified 3 distinct groups of ED patients based on measures of reactive temperament and EC: an overcontrolled/inhibited group (high BIS, low BAS, moderate EC), an undercontrolled/dysregulated group (high BAS, moderate BAS, low EC), and a resilient group (high EC, moderate BAS, low BIS). Patients in the undercontrolled/dysregulated group demonstrated more severe symptoms of bulimia, hostility and Cluster B Personality Disorders compared to the other groups.

In a recent study within a community sample of adolescents (14–19 years), Matton et al. [69] examined the moderating role of EC in the association between sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward and the eating styles restrained eating, emotional eating, and external eating as possible eating disorder precursor. Although some evidence was found for an interaction with EC in girls, main effects in both boys and girls were found for sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward. Müller et al. [70] distinguished two temperament subtypes in a sample of treatment-seeking obese individuals (a cluster high in EC and low in punishment and reward sensitivity (reactivity) and a cluster low in EC and high in punishment and reward sensitivity). In the cluster low in EC, more co-morbid psychopathology (ADHD, binge eating, and depressive symptoms) was found.

EC and SUD

A vulnerability to disinhibition also seems to be a core risk factor associated with both the initiation and continuation of SUD [71]. Two neural systems are involved: one involving EC, localized in prefrontal cortical circuitry, and one involving incentive reactivity (reward sensitivity), localized in subcortical circuitry [26]. In a recent study, Santens et al. [72] combined measures of reactive and regulative measures to establish subgroups of adult inpatients with SUDs and could identify a “resilient” group (high EC, low BAS, low BIS), an “anxious” group (low EC, high BIS, moderate BAS), and a “reward-sensitive” group (moderate EC, high BAS, moderate to low BIS). The “anxious” group had the highest levels of clinical symptomatology.

In a longitudinal study that was part of a larger interventional study, Piehler et al. [73] found that higher levels of EC in adolescence (n = 998) were related to less problematic substance use in early adulthood (tobacco and marihuana). Although this effect was not significant for alcohol use, the interaction between EC and exposure to a “substance use lifestyle” was significant for problematic alcohol use in early adulthood. In another study in undergraduates, Wong and Rowland [74] examined the relationship between two motivational orientations (autonomy and controlled orientations within the Self-Determination Theory of Ryan and Deci [75, 76]), the role of EC and substance use among students. Autonomy orientation, also called intrinsic motivation, positively predicted EC, which was associated with a decrease in the expected frequency of drinking. In contrast, controlled orientation negatively predicted EC, which was associated with an increase in the expected frequency of drinking. Controlled orientation or extrinsic motivation also significantly predicted the presence of alcohol-related problems and illicit drug use via EC. Overall, although no main effect was reported, EC significantly mediated the effects of autonomy orientation and controlled orientation on frequency of alcohol use. In another recent study by Walters et al. [77] in 196 young adults, EC was not significantly associated with alcohol consumption. In addition, in a study by the same group, EC exhibited a protective effect, reducing the effects of alcohol intoxication on the likelihood of unprotected intercourse [78]. In a large (n = 188) study among university students, Kahn et al. [79] investigated whether components of EC serve unique roles in moderating the link between reward and punishment sensitivity and substance use behaviors. In this study of emerging adults, inhibitory control (ATQ measured) was the only component of EC that emerged as a significant moderator.

Different studies explored the effect of early family context or a family history of SUD on the risk for developing substance use problems. In a longitudinal study (from age 11 to 27), Mun et al. [80] found that higher levels of adolescent EC were associated with lower problematic substance use.

In their sample of 277 families, Adkison et al. [81] used only behavioral tasks to measure EC (Stroop task, Stop-Go Task, Tower of London). Their results indicate that especially sons of fathers with an alcohol problem have lower levels of EC which makes them particularly vulnerable to poor self-regulatory strategies.

Taken together, the majority of studies exploring the relationship between EC and substance use support the hypothesis that lack of EC is associated with an increased risk for the development of addictive behaviors. However, findings are not consistent, and none of the studies we found included patients with SUD.

EC and ADHD

In the etiology of ADHD, both neurocognitive and temperamental factors have been implicated. However, studies on EC within adult ADHD remain remarkably scarce within the timeframe 2008 and 2018.

Within a large student sample, Graziano et al. [82] showed an association between the severity of ADHD symptoms and risky behavior but only when EC was low. However, it needs to be noted that this study relates to ADHD symptoms (as assessed with a screening instrument) and not to individuals with a formal ADHD diagnosis.

In another study, Burden et al. [83] assessed response inhibition in a sample of young adults diagnosed with ADHD by studying Event Related Potentials (ERP) during a classic Go/No-Go task. In terms of behavioral performance, poorer inhibitory control was evident in participants with childhood ADHD both with and without prenatal alcohol exposure, as reflected in poorer accuracy when attempting to withhold the button press to No-Go. Analysis of the ERPs revealed a markedly diminished P3 difference between No-Go and Go in the young adults with a history of ADHD but no prenatal alcohol exposure (the idiopathic ADHD group). In a study using ERP deviations of electrophysiological measures, Marquardt et al. [84] suggested reduced effortful engagement of attentional and error-monitoring processes in adults with ADHD. Associations between ADHD symptom scores, event-related potential amplitudes, and poorer task performance in the ADHD group further support this notion.

EC and Behavioral Addictions

Given the recent increase of interest in behavioral addictions, we decided to include a broader spectrum of behavioral addictive patterns than currently included in the DSM or ICD classification [85].

As several studies showed an association between EC and CIU and CB, Claes et al. [63] investigated the relationship between CB and CIU in a sample of 60 patients with an ED. They found a significant association between CB and CIU. Concerning reactive temperament, they could not find a significant association between CB/CIU and BIS, but CIU was significantly related to BAS drive and CB almost significantly positively related to BAS fun seeking. Concerning regulative temperament, both CB and CIU were characterized by lack of EC, more specifically by low levels of inhibitory control. In a sample of CB outpatients, however, Müller et al. [86] surprisingly found no association between low EC an CB. They found an association between BAS, high materialism and especially depression scores. Claes et al. [87] found on the other hand in a sample of female patients with an eating disorder a positive correlation between low level of EC and CB.

In CB, a deficit in SR might also be a potential risk factor. Therefore, temperamental features are thought to be underlying factors associated with CB. In a small study, Voth et al. [88] investigated reactive and regulative temperament in a sample of treatment seeking CB patients combining temperament questionnaires and performance-based tasks. In the CB group, they found lower levels of EC (regulative temperament) and higher levels of BIS and BAS (reactive temperament) in comparison with the control group. The CB group performed worse on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) [89] compared to the control group, which suggests a tendency toward disadvantageous decisions and a higher sensitivity to immediate reward in the CB-group that may reflect higher BAS-reactivity. In the Stroop task, no group differences could be found. They found no significant associations between the questionnaire-based results and the outcomes of the behavioral tasks.

EC and Psychotic Disorders or Schizophrenia

Despite the known relevance of attentional impairments in psychotic patients [90], no studies were identified using measures for EC in psychotic patient samples.

EC and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders

In the PubMed search, we did not find studies of OCD.

According to Gray’s personality model, OCD is defined as an anxiety problem driven by the BIS in combination with a lack of attentional control (subscale of EC) [91].

Our literature search yielded 31 studies covering different clinical disorders. Most frequently studied in consecutive order were mood and anxiety, substance use, and ED. Very few studies were found on adult ADHD and none in patients with psychotic disorders. Overall, the results of the included studies point to a role of EC in the pathogenesis of psychiatric disorders. The role of EC was different throughout different studies, i.e. EC as a main effect, as a moderator or as a mediator. Based on these findings, EC seems to confirm its transdiagnostic dimension cutting through both internalizing and externalizing disorder categories. However, caution should be taken in interpretation. By far most of the studies were performed within non-pathological student samples, exploring relations with disorder symptoms – not disorders. Very few studies included individuals with diagnosed disorders.

EC has been identified as a contributor to both adaptive and adverse outcomes in children, adolescents, and adults and this across many domains. EC seems to affect maladjustment by contributing to the processing of information, as well as to the modulation of emotion and behavior. The ability to divert attention from negative thoughts and to focus on affectively neutral or positive thoughts and activities, for example, seems to be important to experience less negative emotion and has been linked to low levels of anger, anxiety, and depression [92-94]. Engaging in a new activity or focusing on new stimuli appears to reduce distress [95, 96]. The ability to focus attention is also likely to aid in planning behavior, and can help to deal with stressful situations. Attentional control may be especially important for reducing internalizing symptoms such as sadness and anxiety. In addition, the ability to effortfully inhibit behavioral impulses is important for reducing externalizing problems [97, 98]. Although it would be interesting to distinguish between the three components of EC (attentional control, inhibitory control, and activation control) in both internalizing and externalizing disorders, in most studies, however, the total sum of EC is used since the general concept of EC is of interest [27] and the alpha coefficients of the subscales are often too low, indicating that they are less reliable than the total score [28]. We only found three studies where analyses were made with the three subcomponents of the EC [23, 79, 87].

How EC deficits contribute to the different pathogenic processes remains to be understood, with some studies indicating a more direct causal relation and others with EC as a mediator factor modifying the effect of other vulnerabilities. Likewise, its role with regard to specific psychopathology versus a common factor influencing severity and chronification of different disorders remains to be explored.

Although not the topic of the current study, the role of EC in the pathogenesis of PD, increasingly, is highlighted. Claes et al. [11] investigated the association of both reactive and regulative (EC) temperament with PD and found that all PD were characterized by low levels of EC, cluster C PD by high BIS and cluster B PD by high BAS. For several PD, BIS and EC interacted: BIS was only related to severe PD pathology if EC was low. Another study found low EC in relation to borderline PD [99]. In an earlier study, De Panfilis et al. [100] showed a specific role for EC as a mediator between rejection sensitivity and its subsequent interpersonal distress on borderline personality features in an adult community sample, suggesting that low SR capacities may enhance interpersonal distress in response to rejection.

The construct of EC based on Rothbart’s temperament model is the same across the different studies. Only assessment methods of EC differed especially concerning the performance-based tasks, while only the ATQ-EC was used as questionnaire. EC can be measured by self-report questionnaires and behavioral/neurocognitive tasks. In the studies cited above, however, the authors concluded that no significant associations could be found between personality (temperament questionnaire) and neuropsychological measures of executive/reactive inhibition [23, 70]. This is in line with the overall findings within the neuropsychological literature indicating that questionnaires and behavioral tasks do not measure the same underlying construct and therefore should not be used interchangeably [23]. Given that many studies included in this review were done within (large) student samples, most of them used mainly self-report scales, i.e. with the ATQ-EC as most prominent. As to the behavioral measures different tasks were used which are characteristically used within the study of inhibitory processes, e.g. Stroop or SST. However, it can be questioned whether these tasks are specifically mapping EC or other dimensions. This question relates to another, i.e. the exact nature or definition of EC and how it differentiates from executive control. Taken together, measuring EC falls into the same limitations that are also prominent in other neuropsychological dimensions. Both the conceptual unity and the use of different instruments highlight the need for a consensus definition and subsequent standardized measuring method highly needed to allow for comparison of findings over different studies.

EC has been largely understudied in adults compared with the neuropsychological research on executive functioning. Some authors, however, call for an integrated model of SR combining the temperament framework using measures of “effortful control” and the “executive function” framework given the conceptual and empirical overlap between EC and executive functioning [16, 18, 49, 101]. Nigg [17] also states that executive functioning, EC, and cognitive control are closely related. Working memory is also part of or closely related to EC [17, 18] possibly due to shared involvement of executive attention. Bridgett et al. [16] also showed that EC is related to the executive function of updating/monitoring information in working memory. They also found that high EC and better updating/monitoring information in working memory were linked to lower dispositional negative affect.

Research further shows that poor effortful/cognitive control is significantly related to reduced structural and neural activity in prefrontal cortices [102], whereby this reduced activity in the PFC correlates with various psychiatric disorders [103]. These findings are consistent with recent neurobiological studies that identified a common neural circuit disruption in cognitive control across psychiatric disorders [104, 105]. The findings of these studies demonstrate a common pattern of disruption across major psychiatric disorders that parallels the “multiple-demand network” observed in intact cognition [104, 105]. The authors suggest that neuropsychological performance, gray matter volume, and functional brain activation converge to implicate transdiagnostic disruptions in neurocircuitries underlying general cognitive capacity.

Many limitations should be taken into account with regard to the interpretation of the results. First of all, we based our search on one database (PubMed). Although we think this is a valid choice given our focus on EC and psychiatric disorders, the use of additional databases might provide a more comprehensive overview. The current work thus cannot be considered as a systematic review but as an exploration of the possible role of EC throughout different diagnostic categories. Second, despite a large number of studies within student and healthy control groups, the paucity of studies focusing on psychiatric disorders in adult individuals is striking. So, findings from these student samples should be very carefully used when reflecting adult psychopathology. Further, we found no study comparing patient groups with different disorders (or temperaments). Most studies are highlighting differences between patient groups and HC. However, exploring differences between targeted patient groups may yield important additional information on the role of EC dimension in the pathogenesis of different disorders. Finally, throughout different studies different conceptualizations for EC are used and often overlap with constructs that are also used in other constructs, i.e. executive functioning. A better delineation between the different neuropsychological constructs and their assessment methods is badly needed to further explore the details and subtleties of the underlying mechanisms in psychopathology [17, 106].

Taken together, the concept of EC can be seen as a universal psychological dimension covering a continuum from normality to psychopathology and this across disease categories. We found indications that EC is an important psychological dimension within adult psychopathology, cutting through the boundaries of both internalizing and externalizing diagnostic categories. Its role may be both directly causal or as a mediator factor influencing severity and prognosis of a specific disorder. Early assessment of EC and early interventions to improve EC might help to avoid or decrease the risk on developing a broad spectrum of psychiatric disorders.

The authors have no ethical conflicts to disclose.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

There is no funding for this study.

Santens E. performed a literature search in the database PubMed, extracted relevant data and provided summaries of the research studies. Santens E. wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and all authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

1.
Insel
T
,
Cuthbert
B
,
Garvey
M
,
Heinssen
R
,
Pine
DS
,
Quinn
K
, et al
Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
2010
Jul
;
167
(
7
):
748
51
.
[PubMed]
0002-953X
2.
Cuthbert
BN
,
Insel
TR
.
Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC
.
BMC Med
.
2013
May
;
11
(
1
):
126
.
[PubMed]
1741-7015
3.
Rothbart
MK
,
Ellis
LK
,
Posner
MI
. Temperament and self-regulation. In:
Vohs
KD
,
Baumeister
RF
, editors
.
Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
. 2nd ed.
New York (NY)
:
The Guilford Press
;
2006
. pp.
441
600
.
4.
Blair
C
,
Ursache
A
. A bidirectional model of executive functions and self-regulation. In:
Vohs
KD
,
Baumeister
RF
, editors
.
Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
. 2nd ed.
New York (NY)
:
The Guilford Press
;
2011
. pp.
300
20
.
5.
Rothbart
MK
,
Ahadi
SA
.
Temperament and the development of personality
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
1994
Feb
;
103
(
1
):
55
66
.
[PubMed]
0021-843X
6.
Corr
PJ
,
McNaughton
N
.
Neuroscience and approach/avoidance personality traits: a two stage (valuation-motivation) approach
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2012
Nov
;
36
(
10
):
2339
54
.
[PubMed]
0149-7634
7.
Gray
JA
.
The neuropsychology of anxiety: an inquiry into the functions of the septal hippocampal system
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
;
1982
.
8.
Carver
CS
,
White
TL
.
Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
1994
;
67
(
2
):
319
33
. 0022-3514
9.
Nigg
JT
.
Temperament and developmental psychopathology
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2006
Mar-Apr
;
47
(
3-4
):
395
422
.
[PubMed]
0021-9630
10.
Ahadi
SA
,
Rothbart
MK
. Temperament, development and the Big Five. The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. In: Halverson CG, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP, editors. Psychology Press. Chapter 10.
2014
:189-257.
11.
Claes
L
,
Vertommen
S
,
Smis
D
,
Bijttebier
P
.
Emotional reactivity and self-regulation in relation to personality disorders
.
Pers Individ Dif
.
2009
Dec
;
47
(
5
):
948
53
. 0191-8869
12.
Rothbart
MK
,
Bates
JE
. Temperament. In:
Damon
W
,
Lerner
R
,
Eisenberg
N
, editors
.
Social, emotional, and personality development
.
Handbook of child psychology
.
Volume 3
. 6th ed.
New York
:
Wiley
;
2006
. pp.
99
166
.
13.
Rothbart
MK
,
Derryberry
D
. Development of individual differences in temperament. In:
Lamb
ME
,
Brown
A
, editors
.
Advances in developmental psychology
.
Volume 1
.
Hillsdale (NJ)
:
Erlbaum
;
1981
. pp.
37
86
.
14.
Henderson
HA
,
Wachs
TD
.
Temperament theory and the study of cognition-emotion interactions across development
.
Dev Rev
.
2007
Sep
;
27
(
3
):
396
427
. 0273-2297
15.
Eisenberg
N
,
Smith
CL
,
Spinrad
TL
. Effortful control: Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In:
Vohs
KD
,
Baumeister
RF
, editors
.
Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
. 2nd ed.
New York (NY)
:
Guilford Press
;
2011
. pp.
259
82
.
16.
Bridgett
DJ
,
Oddi
KB
,
Laake
LM
,
Murdock
KW
,
Bachmann
MN
.
Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity
.
Emotion
.
2013
Feb
;
13
(
1
):
47
63
.
[PubMed]
1528-3542
17.
Nigg
JT
.
Annual Research Review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2017
Apr
;
58
(
4
):
361
83
.
[PubMed]
0021-9630
18.
Zhou
Q
,
Chen
SH
,
Main
A
.
Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function : a call for an integrated model of self-regulation
.
Child Dev Perspect
.
2012
;
6
(
2
):
112
21
. 1750-8592
19.
Rothbart
MK
,
Ellis
LK
,
Rueda
MR
,
Posner
MI
.
Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control
.
J Pers
.
2003
Dec
;
71
(
6
):
1113
43
.
[PubMed]
0022-3506
20.
Evans
DE
,
Rothbart
MK
.
Developing a model for adult temperament
.
J Res Pers
.
2007
Aug
;
41
(
4
):
868
88
. 0092-6566
21.
Wang
FL
,
Chassin
L
,
Eisenberg
N
,
Spinrad
TL
.
Effortful control predicts adolescent antisocial-aggressive behaviors and depressive symptoms: co-occurrence and moderation by impulsivity
.
Child Dev
.
2015
Nov-Dec
;
86
(
6
):
1812
29
.
[PubMed]
0009-3920
22.
Tseng
WL
,
Guyer
AE
,
Briggs-Gowan
MJ
,
Axelson
D
,
Birmaher
B
,
Egger
HL
, et al
Behavior and emotion modulation deficits in preschoolers at risk for bipolar disorder
.
Depress Anxiety
.
2015
May
;
32
(
5
):
325
34
.
[PubMed]
1091-4269
23.
Claes
L
,
Mitchell
JE
,
Vandereycken
W
.
Out of control? Inhibition processes in eating disorders from a personality and cognitive perspective
.
Int J Eat Disord
.
2012
Apr
;
45
(
3
):
407
14
.
[PubMed]
0276-3478
24.
Nigg
JT
,
Silk
KR
,
Stavro
G
,
Miller
T
.
Disinhibition and borderline personality disorder
.
Dev Psychopathol
.
2005
;
17
(
4
):
1129
49
.
[PubMed]
0954-5794
25.
Nigg
JT
.
Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder?
Psychol Bull
.
2001
Sep
;
127
(
5
):
571
98
.
[PubMed]
0033-2909
26.
Zucker
RA
,
Heitzeg
MM
,
Nigg
JT
.
Parsing the undercontrol/disinhibition pathway to substance use disorders: A multilevel developmental problem
.
Child Dev Perspect
.
2011
Dec
;
5
(
4
):
248
55
.
[PubMed]
1750-8592
27.
Rettew
DC
,
McKee
L
.
Temperament and its role in developmental psychopathology
.
Harv Rev Psychiatry
.
2005
Jan-Feb
;
13
(
1
):
14
27
.
[PubMed]
1067-3229
28.
Tortella-Feliu
M
,
Ibanez
I
,
Garcia de la Banda
G
,
Balle
M
,
Aguaya-Siquier
B
,
Morillas-Romero
A
, et al
The Effortful Control Scale for adults: psychometric properties of the Catalan version and its relationship to cognitive emotion regulation
.
An Psicol
.
2013
Oct
;
29
(
3
):
953
64
.0212-9728
29.
Rueda
MR
,
Fan
J
,
McCandliss
BD
,
Halparin
JD
,
Gruber
DB
,
Lercari
LP
, et al
Development of attentional networks in childhood
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2004
;
42
(
8
):
1029
40
.
[PubMed]
0028-3932
30.
Rothbart
MK
,
Posner
MI
.
The developing brain in a multitasking world
.
Dev Rev
.
2015
Mar
;
35
:
42
63
.
[PubMed]
0273-2297
31.
Petersen
SE
,
Posner
MI
.
The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after
.
Annu Rev Neurosci
.
2012
;
35
(
1
):
73
89
.
[PubMed]
0147-006X
32.
Botvinick
M
,
Nystrom
LE
,
Fissell
K
,
Carter
CS
,
Cohen
JD
.
Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex
.
Nature
.
1999
Nov
;
402
(
6758
):
179
81
.
[PubMed]
0028-0836
33.
Casey
BJ
,
Durston
S
,
Fossella
JA
.
Evidence for a mechanistic model of cognitive control
.
Clin Neurosci Res
.
2001
Jul
;
1
(
4
):
267
82
. 1566-2772
34.
Bush
G
,
Luu
P
,
Posner
MI
.
Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2000
Jun
;
4
(
6
):
215
22
.
[PubMed]
1364-6613
35.
Whalen
PJ
,
Kagan
J
,
Cook
RG
,
Davis
FC
,
Kim
H
,
Polis
S
, et al
Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites
.
Science
.
2004
Dec
;
306
(
5704
):
2061
.
[PubMed]
0036-8075
36.
Ellis
LK
,
Rothbart
MK
. Posner. Temperament and selfregulation. In:
Vohs
KD
,
Baumeister
RF
, editors
.
Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
.
New York (NY)
:
The Guilford Press
;
2004
.
37.
Gerardi-Caulton
G
.
Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self‐regulation in children 24–36 months of age
.
Dev Sci
.
2001
Dec
.1363-755X
38.
Simonds
J
,
Kieras
JE
,
Rueda
R
,
Rothbart
MK
.
Effortful control, executive attention, and emotional regulation in 7–10-year-old children
.
Cogn Dev
.
2007
Oct
;
22
(
4
):
474
88
. 0885-2014
39.
Capaldi
DM
,
Rothbart
MK
.
Development and validation of an early adolescent temperament measure
.
J Early Adolesc
.
1992
May
;
12
(
2
):
153
73
. 0272-4316
40.
Lonigan
CJ
,
Philips
BM
. Temperamental influences on the development of anxiety disorders. In:
Vasey
MW
,
Dadds
MR
, editors
.
The Developmental Psychopatholog of Anxiety
.
Oxford University Press
;
2001
.
41.
Costa
PT
.
MCCRae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual
.
Odessa (FL)
:
Psychological Assessment Resources
;
1992
.
42.
Fan
J
,
McCandliss
BD
,
Sommer
T
,
Raz
A
,
Posner
MI
.
Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2002
Apr
;
14
(
3
):
340
7
.
[PubMed]
0898-929X
43.
Kochanska
G
,
Murray
KT
,
Harlan
ET
.
Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development
.
Dev Psychol
.
2000
Mar
;
36
(
2
):
220
32
.
[PubMed]
0012-1649
44.
Nigg
JT
.
On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy
.
Psychol Bull
.
2000
Mar
;
126
(
2
):
220
46
.
[PubMed]
0033-2909
45.
Logan
GD
,
Cowan
WB
.
On the ability to inhibit thought and action. A theory of an act of control
.
Psychol Rev
.
1984
;
91
(
3
):
295
327
. 0033-295X
46.
Eriksen
BA
,
Eriksen
CW
.
Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target in a nonsearch task
.
Percept Psychophys
.
1974
;
16
(
1
):
143
9
. 0031-5117
47.
Golden
CJ
.
Identification of brain disorders by the Stroop color and word test
.
J Clin Psychol
.
1976
Jul
;
32
(
3
):
654
8
.
[PubMed]
0021-9762
48.
Sharma
L
,
Markon
KE
,
Clark
LA
.
Toward a theory of distinct types of “impulsive” behaviors: A meta-analysis of self-report and behavioral measures
.
Psychol Bull
.
2014
Mar
;
140
(
2
):
374
408
.
[PubMed]
0033-2909
49.
Lin
B
,
Liew
J
,
Perez
M
.
Measurement of self-regulation in early childhood: relations between laboratory and performance-based measures of effortful control and executive functioning
.
Early Child Res Q
.
2019
;
47
2Q
:
1
8
.
[PubMed]
0885-2006
50.
Samyn
V
,
Roeyers
H
,
Bijttebier
P
,
Rosseel
Y
,
Wiersema
JR
.
Assessing effortful control in typical and atypical development: are questionnaires and neuropsychological measures interchangeable? A latent-variable analysis
.
Res Dev Disabil
.
2015
Jan
;
36C
:
587
99
.
[PubMed]
0891-4222
51.
Muris
P
,
van der Pennen
E
,
Sigmond
R
,
Mayer
B
.
Symptoms of anxiety, depression, and aggression in non-clinical children: relationships with self-report and performance-based measures of attention and effortful control
.
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev
.
2008
Dec
;
39
(
4
):
455
67
.
[PubMed]
0009-398X
52.
Oldehinkel
AJ
,
Hartman
CA
,
Ferdinand
RF
,
Verhulst
FC
,
Ormel
J
.
Effortful control as modifier of the association between negative emotionality and adolescents’ mental health problems
.
Dev Psychopathol
.
2007
;
19
(
2
):
523
39
.
[PubMed]
0954-5794
53.
Dinovo
SA
,
Vasey
MW
.
Reactive and self-regulatory dimensions of temperament: interactive relations with symptoms of general distress and anhedonia
.
J Res Pers
.
2011
Oct
;
45
(
5
):
430
40
.
[PubMed]
0092-6566
54.
Tortella-Feliu
M
,
Aguayo
B
,
Sesé
A
,
Morillas-Romero
A
,
Balle
M
,
Gelabert
JM
, et al
Effects of temperament and emotion regulation styles in determining negative emotional states
.
Actas Esp Psiquiatr
.
2012
Nov-Dec
;
40
(
6
):
315
22
.
[PubMed]
1578-2735
55.
Nishiguchi
Y
,
Takano
K
,
Tanno
Y
.
The need for cognition mediates and moderates the association between depressive symptoms and impaired effortful control
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2016
Jul
;
241
:
8
13
.
[PubMed]
0165-1781
56.
Clements
AD
,
Bailey
BA
.
The relationship between temperament and anxiety: phase I in the development of a risk screening model to predict stress-related health problems
.
J Health Psychol
.
2010
May
;
15
(
4
):
515
25
.
[PubMed]
1359-1053
57.
Arger
C
,
Sánchez
O
,
Simonson
J
,
Mezulis
A
.
Pathways to depressive symptoms in young adults: examining affective, self-regulatory, and cognitive vulnerability factors
.
Psychol Rep
.
2012
Oct
;
111
(
2
):
335
48
.
[PubMed]
0033-2941
58.
Kanske
P
,
Kotz
SA
.
Effortful control, depression, and anxiety correlate with the influence of emotion on executive attentional control
.
Biol Psychol
.
2012
Sep
;
91
(
1
):
88
95
.
[PubMed]
0301-0511
59.
Marchetti
I
,
Shumake
J
,
Grahek
I
,
Koster
EH
.
Temperamental factors in remitted depression: the role of effortful control and attentional mechanisms
.
J Affect Disord
.
2018
Aug
;
235
:
499
505
.
[PubMed]
0165-0327
60.
Koster
EH
,
De Lissnyder
E
,
Derakshan
N
,
De Raedt
R
.
Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis
.
Clin Psychol Rev
.
2011
Feb
;
31
(
1
):
138
45
.
[PubMed]
0272-7358
61.
Burdick
KE
,
Gunawardane
N
,
Goldberg
JF
,
Halperin
JM
,
Garno
JL
,
Malhotra
AK
.
Attention and psychomotor functioning in bipolar depression
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2009
Apr
;
166
(
2-3
):
192
200
.
[PubMed]
0165-1781
62.
Müller
A
,
Claes
L
,
Mitchell
JE
,
Fischer
J
,
Horbach
T
,
de Zwaan
M
.
Binge eating and temperament in morbidly obese prebariatric surgery patients
.
Eur Eat Disord Rev
.
2012
Jan
;
20
(
1
):
e91
5
.
[PubMed]
1072-4133
63.
Claes
L
,
Müller
A
,
Norré
J
,
Van Assche
L
,
Wonderlich
S
,
Mitchell
JE
.
The relationship among compulsive buying, compulsive internet use and temperament in a sample of female patients with eating disorders
.
Eur Eat Disord Rev
.
2012
Mar
;
20
(
2
):
126
31
.
[PubMed]
1072-4133
64.
Murphy
FC
,
Sahakian
BJ
,
Rubinsztein
JS
,
Michael
A
,
Rogers
RD
,
Robbins
TW
, et al
Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression
.
Psychol Med
.
1999
Nov
;
29
(
6
):
1307
21
.
[PubMed]
0033-2917
65.
Burt
NM
,
Boddy
LE
,
Bridgett
DJ
.
Contribution of temperament to eating disorder symptoms in emerging adulthood: additive and interactive effects
.
Eat Behav
.
2015
Aug
;
18
:
30
5
.
[PubMed]
1471-0153
66.
Claes
L
,
Robinson
MD
,
Muehlenkamp
JJ
,
Vandereycken
W
,
Bijttebier
P
.
Differentiating bingeing/purging and restrictive eating disorder subtypes: the roles of temperament, effortful control, and cognitive control
.
Pers Individ Dif
.
2010
Jan
;
48
(
2
):
166
70
. 0191-8869
67.
Zwaan
M
,
Engeli
S
,
Müller
A
.
Temperamental factors in severe weight cycling. A cross-sectional study
.
Appetite
.
2015
Aug
;
91
:
336
42
.
[PubMed]
0195-6663
68.
Turner
BJ
,
Claes
L
,
Wilderjans
TF
,
Pauwels
E
,
Dierckx
E
,
Chapman
AL
, et al
Personality profiles in Eating Disorders: further evidence of the clinical utility of examining subtypes based on temperament
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2014
Sep
;
219
(
1
):
157
65
.
[PubMed]
0165-1781
69.
Matton
A
,
Goossens
L
,
Vervaet
M
,
Braet
C
.
Effortful control as a moderator in the association between punishment and reward sensitivity and eating styles in adolescent boys and girls
.
Appetite
.
2017
Apr
;
111
:
177
86
.
[PubMed]
0195-6663
70.
Müller
A
,
Claes
L
,
Wilderjans
TF
,
de Zwaan
M
.
Temperament subtypes in treatment seeking obese individuals: a latent profile analysis
.
Eur Eat Disord Rev
.
2014
Jul
;
22
(
4
):
260
6
.
[PubMed]
1072-4133
71.
Yücel
M
,
Oldenhof
E
,
Ahmed
SH
,
Belin
D
,
Billieux
J
,
Bowden-Jones
H
, et al
A transdiagnostic dimensional approach towards a neuropsychological assessment for addiction: an international Delphi consensus study
.
Addiction
.
2018
Aug
.
[PubMed]
0965-2140
72.
Santens
E
,
Claes
L
,
Dierckx
E
,
Luyckx
K
,
Peuskens
H
,
Dom
G
.
Personality profiles in substance use disorders: do they differ in clinical symptomatology, personality disorders and coping?
Pers Individ Dif
.
2018
Sep
;
131
:
61
6
. 0191-8869
73.
Piehler
TF
,
Véronneau
MH
,
Dishion
TJ
.
Substance use progression from adolescence to early adulthood: effortful control in the context of friendship influence and early-onset use
.
J Abnorm Child Psychol
.
2012
Oct
;
40
(
7
):
1045
58
.
[PubMed]
0091-0627
74.
Wong
MM
,
Rowland
SE
.
Self-determination and substance use: is effortful control a mediator?
Alcohol Clin Exp Res
.
2013
Jun
;
37
(
6
):
1040
7
.
[PubMed]
0145-6008
75.
Ryan
RM
,
Deci
EL
.
Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being
.
Am Psychol
.
2000
Jan
;
55
(
1
):
68
78
.
[PubMed]
0003-066X
76.
Deci
EL
,
Ryan
RM
.
The general causality orientations scale: self-determination in personality
.
J Res Pers
.
1985
;
19
(
2
):
109
34
. 0092-6566
77.
Walters
KJ
,
Simons
JS
,
Simons
RM
. Self-control demands and alcohol-related problems: within- and between-person associations. Psychol Addict Behav.
2018
Sep; 32(6).573–82.
78.
Simons
JS
,
Simons
RM
,
Maisto
SA
,
Hahn
AM
,
Walters
KJ
.
Daily associations between alcohol and sexual behavior in young adults
.
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol
.
2018
Feb
;
26
(
1
):
36
48
.
[PubMed]
1064-1297
79.
Kahn
RE
,
Chiu
PH
,
Deater-Deckard
K
,
Hochgraf
AK
.
King-Casas b, Kim-Spoon J. The interaction between punishment senstivity and effortful control for emerging adults’ substance use behaviors
.
Subst Use Misuse
.
2018
Jul
;
53
(
8
):
1299
310
.
[PubMed]
1082-6084
80.
Mun
CJ
,
Dishion
TJ
,
Tein
JY
,
Otten
R
.
Adolescence effortful control as a mediator between family ecology and problematic substance use in early adulthood: A 16-year prospective study
.
Dev Psychopathol
.
2018
Oct
;
30
(
4
):
1355
69
.
[PubMed]
0954-5794
81.
Adkison
SE
,
Grohman
K
,
Colder
CR
,
Leonard
K
,
Orrange-Torchia
T
,
Peterson
E
, et al
Impact of fathers’ alcohol problems on the development of effortful control in early adolescence
.
J Stud Alcohol Drugs
.
2013
Sep
;
74
(
5
):
674
83
.
[PubMed]
1937-1888
82.
Graziano
PA
,
Reid
A
,
Slavec
J
,
Paneto
A
,
McNamara
JP
,
Geffken
GR
.
ADHD symptomatology and risky health, driving, and financial behaviors in college: the mediating role of sensation seeking and effortful control
.
J Atten Disord
.
2015
Mar
;
19
(
3
):
179
90
.
[PubMed]
1087-0547
83.
Burden
MJ
,
Jacobson
JL
,
Westerlund
A
,
Lundahl
LH
,
Morrison
A
,
Dodge
NC
, et al
An event-related potential study of response inhibition in ADHD with and without prenatal alcohol exposure
.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res
.
2010
Apr
;
34
(
4
):
617
27
.
[PubMed]
0145-6008
84.
Marquardt
L
,
Eichele
H
,
Lundervold
AJ
,
Haavik
J
,
Eichele
T
.
Event-related potential (ERP) correlates of performance monitoring in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
.
Front Psychol
.
2018
Apr
;
9
:
485
.
[PubMed]
1664-1078
85.
Mann
K
,
Kiefer
F
,
Schellekens
A
,
Dom
G
.
Behavioural addictions: classification and consequences
.
Eur Psychiatry
.
2017
Jul
;
44
:
187
8
.
[PubMed]
0924-9338
86.
Müller
A
,
Claes
L
,
Georgiadou
E
,
Möllenkamp
M
,
Voth
EM
,
Faber
RJ
, et al
Is compulsive buying related to materialism, depression or temperament? Findings from a sample of treatment-seeking patients with CB
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2014
Apr
;
216
(
1
):
103
7
.
[PubMed]
0165-1781
87.
Claes
L
,
Bijttebier
P
,
Mitchell
JE
,
de Zwaan
M
,
Mueller
A
.
The relationship between compulsive buying, eating disorder symptoms, and temperament in a sample of female students
.
Compr Psychiatry
.
2011
Jan-Feb
;
52
(
1
):
50
5
.
[PubMed]
0010-440X
88.
Voth
EM
,
Claes
L
,
Georgiadou
E
,
Selle
J
,
Trotzke
P
,
Brand
M
, et al
Reactive and regulative temperament in patients with compulsive buying and non-clinical controls measured by self-report and performance-based tasks
.
Compr Psychiatry
.
2014
Oct
;
55
(
7
):
1505
12
.
[PubMed]
0010-440X
89.
Bechara
A
,
Damasio
AR
,
Damasio
H
,
Anderson
SW
.
Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex
.
Cognition
.
1994
Apr-Jun
;
50
(
1-3
):
7
15
.
[PubMed]
0010-0277
90.
Reichenberg
A
.
The assessment of neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia
.
[Review]
.
Dialogues Clin Neurosci
.
2010
;
12
(
3
):
383
92
.
[PubMed]
1294-8322
91.
Brunfaut
E
,
Claes
L
,
Demyttenaere
K
,
Gabriels
L
,
Hermans
D
. Metacognition and reactive/regulative aspects of temperament in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Metacognition: New Research Developments
.
Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
;
2009
.
92.
Derryberry
D
,
Reed
MA
.
Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
2002
May
;
111
(
2
):
225
36
.
[PubMed]
0021-843X
93.
Derryberry
D
,
Rothbart
MK
.
Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
1988
Dec
;
55
(
6
):
958
66
.
[PubMed]
0022-3514
94.
Silk
JS
,
Steinberg
L
,
Morris
AS
.
Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior
.
Child Dev
.
2003
Nov-Dec
;
74
(
6
):
1869
80
.
[PubMed]
0009-3920
95.
Erber
R
,
Tesser
A
.
Task effort and the regulation of mood: the absorption hypothesis
.
J Exp Soc Psychol
.
1992
Jul
;
28
(
4
):
339
59
. 0022-1031
96.
Harman
C
,
Rothbart
MK
,
Posner
MI
.
Distress and attention interactions in early infancy
.
Motiv Emot
.
1997
;
21
(
1
):
27
43
.0146-7239
97.
Eisenberg
N
,
Fabes
RA
,
Guthrie
IK
,
Reiser
M
.
Dispositional emotionality and regulation: their role in predicting quality of social functioning
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2000
Jan
;
78
(
1
):
136
57
.
[PubMed]
0022-3514
98.
Eisenberg
N
,
Valiente
C
,
Spinrad
TL
,
Liew
J
,
Zhou
Q
,
Losoya
S
,
Reiser
M
,
Cumberland
A
. Dev Psychol. Longitudinal relations of childrens’ effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems.
2009
Jul;45(4):988-1008.
99.
Tortella-Feliu
M
,
Soler
J
,
Burns
L
,
Cebolla
A
,
Elices
M
,
Pascual
JC
, et al
Relationship between effortful control and facets of mindfulness in meditators, non-meditators and individuals with borderline personality disorder
.
Pers Ment Health
.
2018
Aug
;
12
(
3
):
265
78
.
[PubMed]
1932-8621
100.
De Panfilis
C
,
Meehan
KB
,
Cain
NM
,
Clarkin
JF
.
Effortful control, rejection sensitivity, and borderline personality disorder features in adulthood
.
J Pers Disord
.
2016
Oct
;
30
(
5
):
595
612
.
[PubMed]
0885-579X
101.
Kim-Spoon
J
,
Deater-Deckard
K
,
Calkins
SD
,
King-Casas
B
,
Bell
MA
.
Commonality between executive functioning and effortful control related to adjustment
.
J Appl Dev Psychol
.
2019
Jan-Feb
;
60
:
47
55
.
[PubMed]
0193-3973
102.
Banich
MT
.
Executive Function: the search for an integrated account
.
Curr Dir Psychol Sci
.
2009
Apr
;
18
(
2
):
89
94
. 0963-7214
103.
Goodkind
M
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Oathes
DJ
,
Jiang
Y
,
Chang
A
,
Jones-Hagata
LB
, et al
Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness
.
JAMA Psychiatry
.
2015
Apr
;
72
(
4
):
305
15
.
[PubMed]
2168-622X
104.
McTeague
LM
,
Goodkind
MS
,
Etkin
A
.
Transdiagnostic impairment of cognitive control in mental illness
.
J Psychiatr Res
.
2016
Dec
;
83
:
37
46
.
[PubMed]
0022-3956
105.
McTeague
LM
,
Huemer
J
,
Carreon
DM
,
Jiang
Y
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Etkin
A
.
Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in cognitive control across psychiatric disorders
.
Am J Psychiatry
.
2017
Jul
;
174
(
7
):
676
85
.
[PubMed]
0002-953X
106.
Eisenberg
N
.
Commentary: what’s in a word (or words) - on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology - reflections on Nigg (2017)
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2017
Apr
;
58
(
4
):
384
6
.
[PubMed]
0021-9630
Copyright / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Copyright: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be translated into other languages, reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, microcopying, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.