Introduction: Parkinson’s disease (PD) reduces an individual’s capacity for automaticity which limits their ability to perform two tasks simultaneously, negatively impacting daily function. Understanding the neural correlates of dual tasks (DTs) may pave the way for targeted therapies. To better understand automaticity in PD, we aimed to explore whether individuals with differing DT performances possessed differences in brain morphologic characteristics. Methods: Data were obtained from 34 individuals with PD and 47 healthy older adults including (1) demographics (age, sex), (2) disease severity (Movement Disorder Society – Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale [MDS-UPDRS], Hoehn and Yahr, levodopa equivalent daily dose [LEDD]), (3) cognition (Montreal Cognitive Assessment), (4) LEDD, (5) single-task and DT performance during a DT-timed-up-and-go test utilizing a serial subtraction task, and (6) cortical thicknesses and subcortical volumes obtained from volumetric MRI. Participants were categorized as low or high DT performers if their combined DT effect was greater than the previously determined mean value for healthy older adults (μ = –74.2). Nonparametric testing using Quade’s ANCOVA was conducted to compare cortical thicknesses and brain volumes between the highDT and lowDT groups while controlling for covariates: age, sex, MDS-UPDRS part III, LEDD, and intracranial volume. Secondarily, similar comparisons were made between the healthy older adult group and the highDT and lowDT groups. Lastly, a hierarchical linear regression model was conducted regressing combined DT effect on covariates (block one) and cortical thicknesses (block 2) in stepwise fashion. Results: The highDT group had thicker cortices than the lowDT group in the right primary somatosensory cortex (p = 0.001), bilateral primary motor cortices (p ≤ 0.001, left; p = 0.002, right), bilateral supplementary motor areas (p = 0.001, left; p < 0.001, right), and mean of the bilateral hemispheres (p = 0.001, left; p < 0.001, right). Of note, left primary cortex thickness (p = 0.002), left prefrontal cortex thickness (p < 0.001), and right supplementary motor area thickness (p = 0.003) differed when adding a healthy comparison group. Additionally, the regression analysis found that the left paracentral lobule thickness explained 20.8% of the variability in combined DT effect (p = 0.011) beyond the influence of covariates. Conclusions: These results suggest regions underlying DT performance, specifically, a convergence of neural control relying on sensorimotor integration, motor planning, and motor activation to achieve higher levels of DT performance for individuals with PD.

Individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD) have impaired automaticity, the ability to execute tasks without attention directed toward the task [1]. As a result, these individuals have limited dual-task (DT) performance, which is the skilled execution of two tasks simultaneously [1]. During daily activities, this ability is necessary for daily functions (e.g., ambulation or transfers) as they often are completed in the context of walking and attending to a secondary task like talking, manipulation of objects like a cell phone, or monitoring the environment [2]. It is hypothesized that many individuals with PD are impaired in this ability due to insufficient capacity to automatically process a task, as well as limited attentional resources [1]. Individuals with PD possess a lower capacity for automaticity and also poor coordination, balance deficits, bradykinesia/hypokinesia, freezing of gait, and cognitive deficits all of which worsen when confronted with complex or challenging tasks and environments, such as DTs [1, 3].

Performance of motor tasks, including gait, is less consistent and more effortful for individuals with PD when simultaneously required to perform a secondary motor or cognitive task [4]. Prior studies have shown that the performance of a DT leads to alterations in the activity levels in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), premotor cortex (PMC), and supplementary motor area (SMA) in healthy adults as well as those with neurologic disease [5, 6]. Among individuals with PD, similar to healthy adults and individuals with other neurologic diseases, brain activity in the PFC, PMC, and SMA is increased during DT paradigms [7, 8]. Additionally, specific syndromes in PD, such as freezing of gait or mild cognitive impairment, have been linked to the interaction and combined deterioration between motor, cognitive, and limbic functions [9, 10]. Specifically, cortical thicknesses in sensorimotor regions have been found to predict DT cost on gait speed among individuals without freezing of gait, while cortical thicknesses in frontal and parietal areas predict the same in individuals with freezing of gait [10]. Furthermore, dissimilar patterns of subcortical degeneration have been shown to explain differences in symptoms between tremor dominant and postural instability/gait difficulty subtypes such as gait impairment and cognition [11]. These symptoms exert a strong influence on the course of PD progression and the quality of life of those living with this disease. A comprehensive understanding of the neurologic underpinnings of DT abilities may provide insights leading to specifically targeted rehabilitation for individuals with these debilitating syndromes [12]. However, the brain morphology that may accompany or precipitate these impairments in DT performance among individuals with PD has not been fully described. Therefore, identifying regions that differ between individuals that are impaired and those that perform similar to healthy individuals could also provide targets for developing interventions that could attenuate the loss of automaticity during the progression of PD [13]. To this end, the purpose of this study was to determine the differences in cortical thickness and subcortical volumes between individuals who are high DT (highDT) performers and low DT (lowDT) performers in PD. It was hypothesized that the highDT group would exhibit significantly different patterns of volume and cortical thickness in the PFC, SMA, and entorhinal cortex, and that the cortical thickness of these regions would correlate with DT performance. Additionally, it was hypothesized that the highDT group would exhibit significant different patterns of volume in subcortical regions including the caudate, putamen, brainstem, and cerebellar cortex compared to lowDT group.

Design

Exploratory analyses of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data were obtained from 42 people with PD and 47 older adults without a neurological diagnosis. These individuals were recruited as part of an observational study at the Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health [14]. Data obtained and analyzed included the following: demographics (age, sex, race, ethnicity), disease severity (Movement Disorder Society – Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale [MDS-UPDRS], Hoehn and Yahr stage, levodopa equivalent daily dose [LEDD], time since diagnosis, Montreal Cognitive Assessment [MoCA]), single-task (ST) and DT performance obtained during DT timed up and go (TUG) utilizing a serial subtraction task, and cortical volumes obtained from volumetric MRI on a 3T Siemens Skyra scanner. All data were obtained with participants in the clinically defined “ON” state. The “ON” state is defined as when PD medications begin to take effect with patients experiencing period of good symptom control.

Sample and Groups

This study is a secondary analysis of data from a prior study, which was conducted under Institutional Review Board approval. Additionally, each participant provided consent prior to their initiation of the parent study from which these data originate. Inclusion criteria were the following: neurologist-diagnosed PD as per the UK Brain Bank criteria [15] and between the ages of 50 and 85 years old. Participants were excluded if they had other neurologic diagnoses (e.g., stroke, multiple sclerosis), significant orthopedic injury or surgery in the prior 6 months that negatively affected gait or balance, history of deep brain stimulator placement, or an implant that was not MRI compatible. Participants who were left-handed (n = 3) were not included in the analysis to reduce the impact of variations in brain lateralization between individuals who are right-handed and those who are left-handed [16]. Additionally, 3 participants were excluded as they were missing values for variables included in the analyses. A total of 34 participants were included in substantive analyses and dichotomized into the lowDT group (n = 11) or highDT group (n = 23) by comparing their combined DT effect (cDTE) (explained below), a measure of automaticity and DT capacity, to a previously determined mean value for healthy older adults (μ = −74.2%). This cut point was selected as it provided a unique ability to interpret findings relative to healthy individuals of similar age that had minimal to no DT impairment [17]. If a participant’s cDTE was greater than the mean value for healthy older adults, they were included in the highDT group. If a participant’s cDTE value was below the mean, they were included in the lowDT group. All measurements were done in the clinically defined ON PD medication state on the same day that the MRI scan was completed.

Instrumentation

PD Severity

The Hoehn and Yahr scale, MDS-UPDRS part III, and disease duration were extracted as measures of disease severity. The stage of functional disability in PD was defined with the Hoehn and Yahr scale [18]. Additionally, MDS-UPDRS part III was used to clinically determine disease severity [19, 20].

Cognition

The MoCA was used as a measure of global cognition. It is a brief screening tool for global cognition that was designed to be sensitive to mild cognitive impairment [21]. Additionally, data from more detailed cognitive testing were also extracted including the trail making test parts A and B, judgment of line orientation, and symbol digit modality test. These measures were used to describe executive function, processing speed, inhibition control, and visuospatial perception [22‒24].

Neurobehavioral Characteristics

Depression, anxiety, and apathy were assessed, respectively, via the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), State Trait Anxiety Index (STAI), and single dichotomous response item. The GDS is a reliable and valid measure used to identify depression in older adults [25]. The STAI is considered a reliable and nonspecific measure of negative affectivity in an individual [26].

MRI Imaging and Data Processing

The brain volumes of each participant were obtained from a 3T MRI (Skyra, Siemens Medical Solutions USA Inc., Malvern, PA, USA) with semiautomatic segmentation and quantification of brain regions via the FreeSurfer Image Analysis Suite, version 6.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) [27, 28]. Additional details regarding FreeSurfer processing can be found in online supplementary material 1 (for all online suppl. material, see https://doi.org/10.1159/000540393). MRI scans were conducted immediately before the DT assessment. Head motion during measurement was not quantified, but all scans were visually inspected for quality assurance in FreeSurfer. If any motion was encountered during extraction, the boundaries were manually corrected or the participant was discarded, depending on the degree of the artifact. Consequently, scans from 34 individuals with PD and 47 healthy controls were included after excluding two participants with PD due to segmentation errors. FreeSurfer brain volume measurements were extracted for the different regions of interest (ROIs). The ROIs that were selected for this study were based on findings in the following previous studies: caudate volume [29, 30], putamen volume [31], PFC thickness [29], PMC thickness [6], SMA thickness [30], cerebellar cortex volume [30], primary somatosensory (S1) cortex thickness [32], primary motor (M1) cortex thickness [30, 31], entorhinal cortex [33‒35], and brainstem volume [6, 9, 31, 33, 34, 36]. Mean cerebral cortical thicknesses were also included [6, 9, 31, 33, 34, 36]. Given that DT included a mobility task using the lower extremities, the paracentral lobule cortex thickness was also included.

DT Performance

DT performance was evaluated by having each participant execute a motor task and a cognitive task concurrently. Each task was tested both on its own and while simultaneously doing the other task without preferencing attention to one aspect over the other. Performance of both ST and DT trials was used to calculate automaticity and DT performance. Participants were instructed to “walk as quickly as you can safely” and/or “complete the subtraction task as quickly as you can accurately” in both the ST and DT conditions. The methods of calculating automaticity and DT performance are outlined below.

Motor Task

ST motor performance was assessed by having participants complete a single trial of the TUG test. Motor performance was quantified as seconds to complete the TUG [37].

Cognitive Task

Participants then completed a serial subtraction task while seated to assess their ST cognitive performance. Participants were instructed to perform serial subtraction by three from an arbitrary number between 80 and 100 for 20 s. The number of correct responses in the serial subtraction was recorded, and the correct response rate (the average number of seconds per correct response) was calculated [17].

Dual Task

To assess DT performance, participants completed the TUG again while simultaneously completing the serial subtraction task, described below as DT-TUG [37]. To minimize learning effects, the participants were instructed to begin at a different number between 80 and 100. DT motor performance was quantified as seconds to complete the DT-TUG. DT cognitive performance was quantified as correct response rate (the average number of seconds per correct response) using the time in seconds to complete the DT-TUG [17].

DT Measures

Several metrics of DT performance were calculated from the above test. Task-specific interference for each of the motor and cognitive tasks was calculated as outlined below, using either seconds to complete the TUG (ST) and DT-TUG (DT) for motor DT effect (mDTE) or correct response rate for both the TUG (ST) and DT-TUG (DT) for cognitive DT effect (cogDTE) [17, 38]. A positive value indicates relative improvement on performance under DT conditions (facilitation). A negative value indicates relative deterioration on performance under DT conditions (interference) [17, 38].
Task prioritization, or the degree of attentional prioritization to one or the other of the component tasks of the DT, was assessed using the modified attention allocation index (mAAI) [17, 38]. This was quantified as the cogDTE subtracted from the mDTE. Positive values indicate a shift in attention toward the motor task, whereas negative values indicate a shift in attention toward the cognitive task.

Automaticity

To quantify combined DT ability, the following equation, cDTE, was used in order to describe changes in performance across both the motor and cognitive tasks. It represents automaticity or the capacity for DT performance and accounts for both the motor and cognitive tasks [17].

A negative score indicates poorer DT performance (DT interference) when compared to ST performance. On the contrary, a positive score shows improved DT performance (DT facilitation) under DT conditions when compared to ST performance [38]. As noted above, participants were placed in the lowDT group if their cDTE indicated a decline in performance as compared to ST performance of greater than 74.2%.

Data Analysis

All analyses were conducted using SPSS 29.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Armonk, NY, USA: IBM Corp) with α at 0.05. Groups were compared on demographics and disease severity using independent sample t tests (for continuous variables) or χ2 tests (for categorical or nominal variables). Shapiro-Wilk tests and Levene’s tests were conducted and indicated a lack of normal distribution or homogeneity of variances. Due to this, the substantive analyses proceeded with nonparametric testing using Quade’s ANCOVA to compare cortical thicknesses and brain volumes between the highDT and lowDT groups, while controlling for age, sex, MDS-UPDRS part III, total intracranial volume, and LEDD. Between-group effect sizes (ηp2) were also estimated for each hypothesized ROI. Subsequently, Quade’s ANCOVA was used to compare cortical thicknesses and brain volumes of the ROIs between highDT, lowDT, and a healthy comparison group of older adults while controlling for age, sex, and total intracranial volume. Benjamini-Hochberg corrections were applied to account for multiple comparisons. To further understand the relationship between brain morphology and DT performance, a hierarchical multiple linear regression analysis was conducted. The dependent variable in the model was cDTE. Block 1 of the model included covariates (age, MDS-UPDRS part III, sex, total intracranial volume, and LEDD). In block 2 of the model, the ROIs were entered in a stepwise fashion, with an inclusion threshold of p = 0.05.

Group Characteristics

No differences were found between DT groups on demographics, cognition, or neurobehavioral characteristics (ps > 0.113) (see Table 1). The groups differed on HY with lowDT group having a greater proportion of participants in stage three compared to the highDT group having a greater proportion in stage two (p = 0.012). However, the groups did not differ on MDS-UPDRS part III (p = 0.116) or time since diagnosis (p = 0.567). Unsurprisingly, the DT groups differed in measures of DT performance (DTEs and mAAI) and ST performance, used in the calculation of DT effects (Table 1). When comparing the healthy group to the DT groups, the groups differed on sex with the healthy group having a greater proportion of females compared to either DT group (online suppl. material 2).

Table 1.

Participant characteristics in the highDT and lowDT groups with p values from group comparison (independent sample t tests or Pearson χ2 based on data type) with data reported as means ± standard deviation or counts

HighDT (n = 23)LowDT (n = 11)p value
Demographics 
 Sex Male – 16 Male – 8 0.850 
Female – 7 Female – 3 
 Ethnicity Non-Hispanic – 20 Non-Hispanic – 10 0.782 
Hispanic – 2 Hispanic – 1 
Not reported – 1 
 Age, years 68.8±7.2 69.7±6.2 0.739 
 Race White – 19 White – 9 0.766 
Asian – 3 Asian – 1 
African American – 0 African American – 0 
Not reported – 1 Not reported – 0 
 LEDD, mg 836.4±536.5 819.3±357.0 0.924 
PD severity 
 Time since diagnosis, years 8.8±4.5 7.8±4.2 0.567 
 MDS-UPDRS part III 22.6±9.7 29.2±13.7 0.116 
 Hoehn and Yahr Stage 1 = 0 Stage 1 = 1 0.012 
Stage 2 = 22 Stage 2 = 6 
Stage 3 = 1 Stage 3 = 4 
Cognition 
 MoCA (scale points) 26.0±2.4 24.4±2.7 0.078 
 Trail making test part A, s 48.4±21.7 51.6±16.5 0.330 
 Trail making test part B, s 106.3±58.3 134.1±70.3 0.366 
 Judgment of line orientation (raw score) 23.4±5.1 22.1±4.1 0.141 
 Symbol digit modality test (correct responses) 37.4±12.5 32.2±7.4 0.193 
Neurobehavioral characteristics 
 GDS (scale points) 7.1±7.5 9.7±6.1 0.418 
 STAI (scale points) 31.7±9.5 33.5±10.9 0.967 
 Apathy No – 16 No – 8 0.919 
Yes – 7 Yes – 3 
ST performance 
 TUG 8.9±3.3 18.9±19.7 0.023 
 Serial subtraction correct response rate (seconds/correct response) 3.0±3.8 3.0±1.7 0.999 
DT performance 
mDTE −18.0±13.3 −63.4±23.7 <0.001 
cogDTE 0.64±22.6 −133.2±90.3 <0.001 
mAAI −18.7±32.8 69.7±76.3 <0.001 
 cDTE −15.4±22.7 −294.1±207.6 <0.001 
HighDT (n = 23)LowDT (n = 11)p value
Demographics 
 Sex Male – 16 Male – 8 0.850 
Female – 7 Female – 3 
 Ethnicity Non-Hispanic – 20 Non-Hispanic – 10 0.782 
Hispanic – 2 Hispanic – 1 
Not reported – 1 
 Age, years 68.8±7.2 69.7±6.2 0.739 
 Race White – 19 White – 9 0.766 
Asian – 3 Asian – 1 
African American – 0 African American – 0 
Not reported – 1 Not reported – 0 
 LEDD, mg 836.4±536.5 819.3±357.0 0.924 
PD severity 
 Time since diagnosis, years 8.8±4.5 7.8±4.2 0.567 
 MDS-UPDRS part III 22.6±9.7 29.2±13.7 0.116 
 Hoehn and Yahr Stage 1 = 0 Stage 1 = 1 0.012 
Stage 2 = 22 Stage 2 = 6 
Stage 3 = 1 Stage 3 = 4 
Cognition 
 MoCA (scale points) 26.0±2.4 24.4±2.7 0.078 
 Trail making test part A, s 48.4±21.7 51.6±16.5 0.330 
 Trail making test part B, s 106.3±58.3 134.1±70.3 0.366 
 Judgment of line orientation (raw score) 23.4±5.1 22.1±4.1 0.141 
 Symbol digit modality test (correct responses) 37.4±12.5 32.2±7.4 0.193 
Neurobehavioral characteristics 
 GDS (scale points) 7.1±7.5 9.7±6.1 0.418 
 STAI (scale points) 31.7±9.5 33.5±10.9 0.967 
 Apathy No – 16 No – 8 0.919 
Yes – 7 Yes – 3 
ST performance 
 TUG 8.9±3.3 18.9±19.7 0.023 
 Serial subtraction correct response rate (seconds/correct response) 3.0±3.8 3.0±1.7 0.999 
DT performance 
mDTE −18.0±13.3 −63.4±23.7 <0.001 
cogDTE 0.64±22.6 −133.2±90.3 <0.001 
mAAI −18.7±32.8 69.7±76.3 <0.001 
 cDTE −15.4±22.7 −294.1±207.6 <0.001 

MDS-UPDRS, Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; highDT, high dual-task performers; lowDT, low dual-task performers.

Brain Morphology Differences between the DT Groups

Notably, the highDT group had thicker cerebral cortices in both the left (p = 0.001) and right (p < 0.001) hemispheres grossly. Among the ROIs analyzed, the highDT group had thicker S1 cortices in the right hemisphere (p < 0.001), M1 cortices bilaterally (p < 0.001, left; p = 0.001, right), and SMA cortices bilaterally (p = 0.001, left; p < 0.001, right) (Fig. 1; Table 2). There were no significant differences between groups in any of the subcortical ROIs. Effect size estimates (ηp2) for the differences in cortical thicknesses or brain volumes between the highDT and lowDT groups for each hypothesized ROI are reported in Table 2. Broadly, effect sizes for hypothesized cortical ROIs had moderate to large effect sizes, while subcortical ROIs tended to have moderate to small effect sizes.

Fig. 1.

Violin plots of the ranks of mean hemispheric (a), S1 (b), M1 (c), SMA (d) cortical thicknesses between highDT, lowDT, and healthy groups for left and right hemispheres. Statistically significant p values are presented for differences between highDT and lowDT groups generated from Quade’s ANCOVAs (between two groups). Statistically significant p values presented for differences between healthy group and either DT group are represented from subsequent Quade’s ANCOVAs (between 3 groups).

Fig. 1.

Violin plots of the ranks of mean hemispheric (a), S1 (b), M1 (c), SMA (d) cortical thicknesses between highDT, lowDT, and healthy groups for left and right hemispheres. Statistically significant p values are presented for differences between highDT and lowDT groups generated from Quade’s ANCOVAs (between two groups). Statistically significant p values presented for differences between healthy group and either DT group are represented from subsequent Quade’s ANCOVAs (between 3 groups).

Close modal
Table 2.

Means ± standard deviation, with p values, F statistics, and effect sizes (partial eta squared) for difference in brain volume between highDT and lowDT groups for each hypothesized ROI

HighDT (n = 23)LowDT (n = 11)p valueFηp2
PMC thickness 
 Left 2.39±0.10 2.27±0.22 0.037 4.763 0.159 
 Right 2.46±0.14 2.32±0.16 0.011 7.297 0.217 
Entorhinal cortex thickness 
 Left 3.44±0.30 3.04±0.61 0.013 6.947 0.206 
 Right 3.45±0.30 3.08±0.50 0.027 5.366 0.307 
PFC thickness 
 Left 2.32±0.12 2.27±0.13 0.097 2.916 0.079 
 Right 2.42±0.19 2.34±0.15 0.004a 9.549 0.182 
Paracentral lobule cortical thickness 
 Left 2.40±0.11 2.27±0.15 0.008 8.042 0.416 
 Right 2.35±0.13 2.35±0.13 0.144 2.245 0.320 
S1 cortex thickness 
 Left 1.99±0.12 1.88±0.10 0.025 5.560 0.337 
 Right 2.01±0.10 1.89±0.10 <0.001a 18.079 0.308 
M1 cortex thickness 
 Left 2.46±0.13 2.28±0.16 <0.001a 18.280 0.430 
 Right 2.52±0.11 2.34±0.17 0.001a 14.518 0.344 
SMA cortical thickness 
 Left 2.513±0.09 2.40±0.09 0.001a 13.961 0.451 
 Right 2.57±0.11 2.40±0.11 <0.001a 32.168 0.501 
Mean cerebral cortex thickness 
 Left 2.39±0.07 2.29±0.10 0.001a 12.279 0.480 
 Right 2.40±0.08 2.28±0.10 <0.001a 24.690 0.447 
Caudate volume 
 Left 2,954.4±356.1 3,148.8±269.0 0.087 3.115 0.392 
 Right 3,078.1±428.7 3,348.9±322.0 0.016 6.475 0.415 
Putamen volume 
 Left 4,015.4±525.6 4,192.5±307.6 0.391 0.755 0.222 
 Right 4,005.8±522.9 4,002.3±400.5 0.748 0.105 0.144 
Cerebellar cortex volume 
 Left 48,714.3±5,583.8 52,809.3±5,549.5 0.060 3.799 0.353 
 Right 49,464.1±6,331.2 53,849.7±5,198.1 0.081 3.240 0.362 
Brainstem volume 22,416.0±3,756.0 23,277.9±3,378.7 0.498 0.71 0.097 
HighDT (n = 23)LowDT (n = 11)p valueFηp2
PMC thickness 
 Left 2.39±0.10 2.27±0.22 0.037 4.763 0.159 
 Right 2.46±0.14 2.32±0.16 0.011 7.297 0.217 
Entorhinal cortex thickness 
 Left 3.44±0.30 3.04±0.61 0.013 6.947 0.206 
 Right 3.45±0.30 3.08±0.50 0.027 5.366 0.307 
PFC thickness 
 Left 2.32±0.12 2.27±0.13 0.097 2.916 0.079 
 Right 2.42±0.19 2.34±0.15 0.004a 9.549 0.182 
Paracentral lobule cortical thickness 
 Left 2.40±0.11 2.27±0.15 0.008 8.042 0.416 
 Right 2.35±0.13 2.35±0.13 0.144 2.245 0.320 
S1 cortex thickness 
 Left 1.99±0.12 1.88±0.10 0.025 5.560 0.337 
 Right 2.01±0.10 1.89±0.10 <0.001a 18.079 0.308 
M1 cortex thickness 
 Left 2.46±0.13 2.28±0.16 <0.001a 18.280 0.430 
 Right 2.52±0.11 2.34±0.17 0.001a 14.518 0.344 
SMA cortical thickness 
 Left 2.513±0.09 2.40±0.09 0.001a 13.961 0.451 
 Right 2.57±0.11 2.40±0.11 <0.001a 32.168 0.501 
Mean cerebral cortex thickness 
 Left 2.39±0.07 2.29±0.10 0.001a 12.279 0.480 
 Right 2.40±0.08 2.28±0.10 <0.001a 24.690 0.447 
Caudate volume 
 Left 2,954.4±356.1 3,148.8±269.0 0.087 3.115 0.392 
 Right 3,078.1±428.7 3,348.9±322.0 0.016 6.475 0.415 
Putamen volume 
 Left 4,015.4±525.6 4,192.5±307.6 0.391 0.755 0.222 
 Right 4,005.8±522.9 4,002.3±400.5 0.748 0.105 0.144 
Cerebellar cortex volume 
 Left 48,714.3±5,583.8 52,809.3±5,549.5 0.060 3.799 0.353 
 Right 49,464.1±6,331.2 53,849.7±5,198.1 0.081 3.240 0.362 
Brainstem volume 22,416.0±3,756.0 23,277.9±3,378.7 0.498 0.71 0.097 

HighDT, high dual-task performers; lowDT, low dual-task performers.

aIndicates a statistically significant difference between highDT and lowDT groups after Benjamini-Hochberg correction for multiple comparison.

Brain Morphology Differences between Healthy and DT Groups

Additionally, differences in cortical thickness and brain volumes between highDT, lowDT, and healthy groups for each hypothesized ROI were tested. Of note, left M1 cortex thickness (p = 0.002), left PFC thickness (p < 0.001), and right SMA thickness (p = 0.003) differed between groups (see online suppl. material 3).

Correlation between cDTE and Brain Morphology in Participants with PD

The hierarchal linear regression model found that the included covariates (block 1 – age, sex, total intracranial volume, MDS-UPDRS part III, and LEDD) did not relate to cDTE (p = 0.967). In block 2, the left paracentral lobule thickness was selected stepwise and significantly improved the model fit to the data (ΔF = 7.400, p = 0.011) such that the addition of the paracentral lobule thickness to the model improved the variance explained in cDTE by 20.8% (see Fig. 2). Among the other ROIs, only the left S1 cortex thickness met the threshold for inclusion following block 1 (p = 0.048); however, it did not meet criteria for inclusion in the model after the left paracentral lobule was added to the model.

Fig. 2.

Scatter plot displaying the correlation and line of best fit between left paracentral lobule thickness and cDTE, corrected for age, sex, MDS-UPDRS part III, total intracranial volume, and LEDD.

Fig. 2.

Scatter plot displaying the correlation and line of best fit between left paracentral lobule thickness and cDTE, corrected for age, sex, MDS-UPDRS part III, total intracranial volume, and LEDD.

Close modal

Overall, the results of this study indicate that DT performance in PD may be related to cortical thickness of the sensorimotor and SMA cortices. Specifically, those with better DT performance (highDT group) had larger right S1 cortices, bilateral M1 cortices, and bilateral SMAs. Additionally, average cortical thickness in both hemispheres was greater in the highDT group. We initially hypothesized greater subcortical volumes in highDT performers given the cognitive control required for DT performance, but interestingly, no differences were found between groups on volumes of any subcortical ROIs. Interestingly, this continued to hold true when comparing to healthy older adults. PD pathology directly affects the brainstem, caudate, and putamen and indirectly affects the cerebellum and cerebral white matter throughout its development before PD diagnosis and during the disease post-diagnosis [39]. Given this, it is possible that the effects of PD on these regions drive individuals to compensate with other cortical regions to maintain DT performance. It is also possible that any morphologic changes in these regions associated with DT performance occur early in disease development. This notion is supported by the evidence that DT capacity is limited early in the prodromal state of PD [40]. Of note, while the findings indicate some mixed lateralization of the results, with the highDT group possessing larger S1 cortices only in the right hemisphere (as noted above), bilateral differences for each of the regions were present before correction for multiple comparisons. This loss of statistical difference may be due to insufficient sample size. Given this, we will focus on the regions that were found to be different between groups with less emphasis on laterality.

Statistically significant findings for the cerebral cortex thickness of the entire left and right hemispheres were observed. These were expected, as it has been shown that poorer DT capacity is associated with smaller brain volumes in other populations [41]. The findings of this study extend prior studies and suggest widespread neural involvement in DT performance. This is supported by an extensive body of literature using functional neuroimaging during DTs, which indicates the involvement of multiple diverse regions throughout the cerebral cortex [1, 7, 8].

The findings of this study indicated that individuals in this study with better DT performance had thicker SMA and sensorimotor cortices. Thicker SMA cortices in highDT performers may reflect their role in working memory and the performance of learned voluntary movements [42]. Previous studies have demonstrated increased activation localized to the SMA during DT conditions when compared to ST walking due to its role in motor preparation, planning, and sensorimotor integration [5]. The results of our study extend these findings. Additionally, there is strong evidence from mobile neuroimaging studies to support the role of the SMA in dual-tasking among individuals with PD [7]. Given the complexity of the DT paradigm utilized in the current study, it is unsurprising that SMA thickness was related to DT performance. Additionally, the finding that the highDT group had thicker S1 and M1 cortices could reflect their role in receiving and organizing afferent inputs and generating neural impulses that produce and regulate movement [36]. Greater thickness in both M1 and S1 may allow for greater capacity to integrate sensory information to execute the appropriate motor response.

Notably, the left M1 cortex, left PFC, and right SMA cortex differed in the analyses of differences between DT groups and healthy older adults. Specifically, PFC thicknesses in both the highDT and lowDT groups were significantly different than in the healthy comparison group. Prior research confirms this finding, suggesting that PD affects this region in early stages of the disease [43]. Additionally, lowDT performers had thinner M1 and SMA cortices than both the highDT and healthy comparison groups. Providing further insight into these findings is the multiple linear regression analysis which indicated that the thickness of the paracentral lobule was related to DT performance in people with PD. Greater thickness in the paracentral region may signify more efficiency with motor execution in the lower extremities [44]. This could be related to better automaticity with lower extremity motor tasks and may explain its greater thickness in the highDT group. Additionally, given the findings of the regression analysis, the paracentral region may play a unique and integral role in the execution of DTs for people with PD. This is supported by previous studies that show the impact of PD on the sensorimotor cortex and the role of this brain region in DT performance [7, 36]. However, before this study, morphologic differences associated with DT performance had not been well described in individuals with PD. Taken together, the results of this study provide an initial understanding of the brain morphology changes among individuals with differing DT abilities. This can provide a vital first step toward the development of interventions that could minimize the loss of DT ability and automaticity alongside the progression of PD.

Limitations

While this study provides insight into brain regions correlated with DT performance in PD, it is limited in several ways. First, this study was a secondary analysis; hence, the data were not designed or collected to answer the research questions posed in this study. Consequently, the relatively small sample size may be prone to type II errors. Additionally, participants were divided into lowDT and highDT groups based on the mean value of cDTE from a group of healthy subjects from a previous study, where values were variable (μ = −74.2 ± 131.3). While utilizing this cut point can be a weakness given the wide variability reported in the prior study, the ability to interpret the findings of this study relatively to DT ability of a comparable healthy group is enhanced by this approach. Further research is needed to establish more definitive methods for classifying individuals as highDT or lowDT performers. Another item to consider is the mismatch in time allowed to perform serial subtraction between ST and DT paradigms. While participants were given 20 s to complete the cognitive ST, they were constrained to the duration it took them to complete the DT-TUG to perform the cognitive portion of the DT. Differences in the time each task was performed could have influenced the correct response rate, highlighting a limitation in the methodology of this calculation. This study also examined only one aspect of DT performance (cDTE). Future work could investigate cortical thickness within other aspects of DT performance such as task-specific interference and attention allocation. Additionally, the sample only included people with mild to moderate PD. As such, these results may not be generalizable to individuals with advanced PD. Due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, we cannot conclude that progressive cortical thinning, associated with PD progression, caused the observed differences in DT performance. Future research should utilize longitudinal approaches to better understand the relationship between cortical thinning, DT performance, and PD progression. Specifically, investigating the relationship between brain volumes and other DT variables, including task-specific interference or attention allocation index, may provide valuable insights into the complex interplay between these factors. Future work would also benefit from comparison of DT neural correlates between individuals with healthy adults and those with PD. Last, studies utilizing mobile neuroimaging techniques (e.g., fNIRS, EEG) to study real-time brain function during DT activities in PD could provide further insights into the neural control that automatic and attentional processes contribute to DT performance.

The results of this study suggest a relationship between cortical thinning and automaticity in PD. Overall, DT performance may be related to broad and diverse cortical regions. Specifically, thinner SMA, S1, and M1 cortices were found in those with poorer DT performance in individuals with PD. The morphologic differences in these regions may account for worse DT performance due to decreased motor planning and execution. The influence of the progression of neural degradation throughout PD on DT performance warrants further research.

The authors thank the participants who completed this study.

This study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board on ethical standards on human experimentation at the Cleveland Clinic, Approval No. 15-987. Written informed consent was obtained from participants to participate in this study.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare relevant to the content of this article.

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number P20GM109025. Content is the sole responsibility of the authors and should not be taken to reflect the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

S.C. and J.L. both contributed to the conception and design of this project and the acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of the data. S.C. completed the initial draft, and Y.C., M.L. N.F., V.M, and J.L. critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available at nevadacntn.org.

1.
Wu
T
,
Hallett
M
,
Chan
P
.
Motor automaticity in Parkinson’s disease
.
Neurobiol Dis
.
2015
;
82
:
226
34
.
2.
Deblock-Bellamy
A
,
Lamontagne
A
,
McFadyen
BJ
,
Ouellet
MC
,
Blanchette
AK
.
Dual-Task abilities during activities representative of daily life in community-dwelling stroke survivors: a pilot study
.
Front Neurol
.
2022
;
13
:
855226
.
3.
Zheng
Y
,
Meng
Z
,
Zhi
X
,
Liang
Z
.
Dual-task training to improve cognitive impairment and walking function in Parkinson’s disease patients: a brief review
.
Sports Med Health Sci
.
2021
;
3
(
4
):
202
6
.
4.
Christofoletti
G
,
Andrade
LP
,
Beinotti
F
,
Borges
G
.
Cognition and dual-task performance in older adults with Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease
.
Int J Gen Med
.
2014
;
7
:
383
8
.
5.
Lim
SB
,
Peters
S
,
Yang
CL
,
Boyd
LA
,
Liu-Ambrose
T
,
Eng
JJ
.
Frontal, sensorimotor, and posterior parietal regions are involved in dual-task walking after stroke
.
Front Neurol
.
2022
;
13
:
904145
.
6.
Liu
YC
,
Yang
YR
,
Tsai
YA
,
Wang
RY
,
Lu
CF
.
Brain activation and gait alteration during cognitive and motor dual task walking in stroke-A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study
.
IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng
.
2018
;
26
(
12
):
2416
23
.
7.
Liu
YC
,
Yang
YR
,
Yeh
NC
,
Ku
PH
,
Lu
CF
,
Wang
RY
.
Multiarea brain activation and gait deterioration during a cognitive and motor dual task in individuals with Parkinson disease
.
J Neurol Phys Ther
.
2022
;
46
(
4
):
260
9
.
8.
Maidan
I
,
Hacham
R
,
Galperin
I
,
Giladi
N
,
Holtzer
R
,
Hausdorff
JM
, et al
.
Neural variability in the prefrontal cortex as a reflection of neural flexibility and stability in patients with Parkinson disease
.
Neurology
.
2022
;
98
(
8
):
e839
47
.
9.
Pietracupa
S
,
Suppa
A
,
Upadhyay
N
,
Giannì
C
,
Grillea
G
,
Leodori
G
, et al
.
Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: gray and white matter abnormalities
.
J Neurol
.
2018
;
265
(
1
):
52
62
.
10.
Silva-Batista
C
,
Ragothaman
A
,
Mancini
M
,
Carlson-Kuhta
P
,
Harker
G
,
Jung
SH
, et al
.
Cortical thickness as predictor of response to exercise in people with Parkinson’s disease
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2021
;
42
(
1
):
139
53
.
11.
Rosenberg-Katz
K
,
Herman
T
,
Jacob
Y
,
Kliper
E
,
Giladi
N
,
Hausdorff
JM
.
Subcortical volumes differ in Parkinson’s disease motor subtypes: new insights into the pathophysiology of disparate symptoms
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2016
;
10
:
356
.
12.
Mancini
M
,
Bloem
BR
,
Horak
FB
,
Lewis
SJG
,
Nieuwboer
A
,
Nonnekes
J
.
Clinical and methodological challenges for assessing freezing of gait: future perspectives
.
Mov Disord
.
2019
;
34
(
6
):
783
90
.
13.
Armstrong
NM
,
An
Y
,
Shin
JJ
,
Williams
OA
,
Doshi
J
,
Erus
G
, et al
.
Associations between cognitive and brain volume changes in cognitively normal older adults
.
Neuroimage
.
2020
;
223
:
117289
.
14.
Ritter
A
,
Cummings
J
,
Nance
C
,
Miller
JB
.
Neuroscience learning from longitudinal cohort studies of Alzheimer’s disease: lessons for disease-modifying drug programs and an introduction to the Center for Neurodegeneration and Translational Neuroscience
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2018
;
4
(
1
):
350
6
.
15.
Hughes
AJ
,
Daniel
SE
,
Kilford
L
,
Lees
AJ
.
Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases
.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
.
1992
;
55
(
3
):
181
4
.
16.
Li
M
,
Chen
H
,
Wang
J
,
Liu
F
,
Long
Z
,
Wang
Y
, et al
.
Handedness- and hemisphere-related differences in small-world brain networks: a diffusion tensor imaging tractography study
.
Brain Connect
.
2014
;
4
(
2
):
145
56
.
17.
Longhurst
JK
,
Rider
JV
,
Cummings
JL
,
John
SE
,
Poston
B
,
Held-Bradford
E
, et al
.
A novel way of measuring dual task interference: the reliability and construct validity of the dual task effect battery in healthy adults and individuals with neurodegenerative disease
.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair
.
2022
;
36
(
6
):
346
59
.
18.
Rabey
JM
,
Korczyn
AD
.
The hoehn and yahr rating scale for Parkinson’s disease
. In:
Przuntek
H
,
Kraus
PH
,
Klotz
P
,
Korczyn
AD
, editors.
Instrumental methods and scoring in extrapyramidal disorders
.
Berlin, Heidelberg
:
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
;
1995
. p.
7
17
.
19.
Martínez-Martín
P
,
Rodríguez-Blázquez
C
,
Mario
A
,
Arakaki
T
,
Arillo
VC
,
Chaná
P
, et al
.
Parkinson’s disease severity levels and MDS-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale
.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord
.
2015
;
21
(
1
):
50
4
.
20.
Goetz
CG
,
Tilley
BC
,
Shaftman
SR
,
Stebbins
GT
,
Fahn
S
,
Martinez-Martin
P
, et al
.
Movement disorder society-sponsored revision of the unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results
.
Mov Disord
.
2008
;
23
(
15
):
2129
70
.
21.
Nasreddine
ZS
,
Phillips
NA
,
Bedirian
V
,
Charbonneau
S
,
Whitehead
V
,
Collin
I
, et al
.
The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2005
;
53
(
4
):
695
9
.
22.
Llinàs-Reglà
J
,
Vilalta-Franch
J
,
López-Pousa
S
,
Calvó-Perxas
L
,
Torrents Rodas
D
,
Garre-Olmo
J
.
The trail making test
.
Assessment
.
2017
;
24
(
2
):
183
96
.
23.
Treccani
B
,
Cubelli
R
.
Spatial asymmetries undermine also the short forms of the Judgement of Line Orientation test
.
Neuropsychology
.
2019
;
33
(
3
):
301
8
.
24.
Fellows
RP
,
Schmitter-Edgecombe
M
.
Symbol digit modalities test: regression-based normative data and clinical utility
.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol
.
2019
;
35
(
1
):
105
15
.
25.
Yesavage
JA
,
Brink
TL
,
Rose
TL
,
Lum
O
,
Huang
V
,
Adey
M
, et al
.
Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report
.
J Psychiatr Res
.
1982
;
17
(
1
):
37
49
.
26.
Knowles
KA
,
Olatunji
BO
.
Specificity of trait anxiety in anxiety and depression: meta-analysis of the state-trait anxiety inventory
.
Clin Psychol Rev
.
2020
;
82
:
101928
.
27.
Fischl
B
,
Salat
DH
,
Busa
E
,
Albert
M
,
Dieterich
M
,
Haselgrove
C
, et al
.
Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain
.
Neuron
.
2002
;
33
(
3
):
341
55
.
28.
Fischl
B
,
Salat
DH
,
van der Kouwe
AJ
,
Makris
N
,
Segonne
F
,
Quinn
BT
, et al
.
Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images
.
Neuroimage
.
2004
;
23
(
Suppl 1
):
S69
84
.
29.
Preziosa
P
,
Rocca
MA
,
Pagani
E
,
Valsasina
P
,
Amato
MP
,
Brichetto
G
, et al
.
Structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of fatigue and dual-task performance in progressive multiple sclerosis
.
J Neurol
.
2023
;
270
(
3
):
1543
63
.
30.
Sarasso
E
,
Gardoni
A
,
Piramide
N
,
Volontè
MA
,
Canu
E
,
Tettamanti
A
, et al
.
Dual-task clinical and functional MRI correlates in Parkinson’s disease with postural instability and gait disorders
.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord
.
2021
;
91
:
88
95
.
31.
Wu
T
,
Long
X
,
Wang
L
,
Hallett
M
,
Zang
Y
,
Li
K
, et al
.
Functional connectivity of cortical motor areas in the resting state in Parkinson’s disease
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2011
;
32
(
9
):
1443
57
.
32.
Nelson
AJ
,
Hoque
T
,
Gunraj
C
,
Chen
R
.
Altered somatosensory processing in Parkinson’s disease and modulation by dopaminergic medications
.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord
.
2018
;
53
:
76
81
.
33.
Goldman
JG
,
Stebbins
GT
,
Bernard
B
,
Stoub
TR
,
Goetz
CG
,
deToledo-Morrell
L
.
Entorhinal cortex atrophy differentiates Parkinson’s disease patients with and without dementia
.
Mov Disord
.
2012
;
27
(
6
):
727
34
.
34.
Jia
X
,
Wang
Z
,
Yang
T
,
Li
Y
,
Gao
S
,
Wu
G
, et al
.
Entorhinal cortex atrophy in early, drug-naive Parkinson’s disease with mild cognitive impairment
.
Aging Dis
.
2019
;
10
(
6
):
1221
32
.
35.
Sakurai
R
,
Bartha
R
,
Montero-Odasso
M
.
Entorhinal cortex volume is associated with dual-task gait cost among older adults with MCI: results from the gait and brain study
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
.
2019
;
74
(
5
):
698
704
.
36.
Burciu
RG
,
Vaillancourt
DE
.
Imaging of motor cortex physiology in Parkinson’s disease
.
Mov Disord
.
2018
;
33
(
11
):
1688
99
.
37.
Shumway-Cook
A
,
Brauer
S
,
Woollacott
M
.
Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the Timed up & Go Test
.
Phys Ther
.
2000
;
80
(
9
):
896
903
.
38.
Kelly
VE
,
Janke
AA
,
Shumway-Cook
A
.
Effects of instructed focus and task difficulty on concurrent walking and cognitive task performance in healthy young adults
.
Exp Brain Res
.
2010
;
207
(
1–2
):
65
73
.
39.
Braak
H
,
Tredici
KD
,
Rüb
U
,
de Vos
RA
,
Jansen Steur
EN
,
Braak
E
.
Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease
.
Neurobiol Aging
.
2003
;
24
(
2
):
197
211
.
40.
Belghali
M
,
Chastan
N
,
Davenne
D
,
Decker
LM
.
Improving dual-task walking paradigms to detect prodromal Parkinson’s and alzheimer’s diseases
.
Front Neurol
.
2017
;
8
:
207
.
41.
Longhurst
JK
,
Wise
MA
,
Krist
DJ
,
Moreland
CA
,
Basterrechea
JA
,
Landers
MR
.
Brain volumes and dual-task performance correlates among individuals with cognitive impairment: a retrospective analysis
.
J Neural Transm
.
2020
;
127
(
7
):
1057
71
.
42.
du Boisgueheneuc
F
,
Levy
R
,
Volle
E
,
Seassau
M
,
Duffau
H
,
Kinkingnehun
S
, et al
.
Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: a lesion study
.
Brain
.
2006
;
129
(
Pt 12
):
3315
28
.
43.
Pletcher
C
,
Dabbs
K
,
Barzgari
A
,
Pozorski
V
,
Haebig
M
,
Wey
S
, et al
.
Cerebral cortical thickness and cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease
.
Cereb Cortex Commun
.
2023
;
4
(
1
):
tgac044
.
44.
Debaere
F
,
Wenderoth
N
,
Sunaert
S
,
Van Hecke
P
,
Swinnen
SP
.
Cerebellar and premotor function in bimanual coordination: parametric neural responses to spatiotemporal complexity and cycling frequency
.
Neuroimage
.
2004
;
21
(
4
):
1416
27
.