The core knowledge (CK) account of human development ascribes higher-order cognition to infants on the basis of looking time measures. In this paper, we investigate the conceptual foundations of this account through an examination of the preferential looking paradigm. We focus on the use of this paradigm in social cognitive and morality research, which involves ascriptions of expectation, surprise, preference, belief understanding, and moral judgment to infant looking behavior. We compare CK researchers' usage of these terms with everyday usage, and conclude that the application of belief and morality to infant looking behavior is overzealous. Based on these considerations, we argue that a developmental systems approach may provide a more appropriate theoretical framework for studying the development of such capacities.

1.
Allen, J.W.P., & Bickhard, M.H. (2013). Stepping off the pendulum: Why only an action-based approach can transcend the nativist-empiricist debate. Cognitive Development, 28, 96-133.
2.
Aslin, R.N. (2000). Why take the cog out of infant cognition? Infancy, 1, 463-470.
3.
Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½- and 4½-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655-664.
4.
Baillargeon, R. (2000). Reply to Bogartz, Shinskey, and Schilling; Schilling; and Cashon and Cohen. Infancy, 1, 447-442.
5.
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R.M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 110-118.
6.
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E.S., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191-208.
7.
Bloom, P. (2012). Moral nativism and moral psychology. In M. Mikulincer & P. Shaver (Eds.), The social psychology of morality: Exploring the causes of good and evil (pp. 71-89). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
8.
Bogartz, R.S., Shinskey, J.L., & Schilling, T.H. (2000). Object permanence in five-and-a-half-month-old infants? Infancy, 1, 403-428.
9.
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-olds show false belief understanding in the active helping paradigm. Cognition, 12, 337-342.
10.
Cohen, L.B. (2004). Uses and misuses of habituation and related preference paradigms. Infant and Child Development, 13, 349-352.
11.
Colombo, J., & Mitchell, D.W. (2009). Infant visual habituation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92, 225-234.
12.
Dahl, A. Schuck, R.K., & Campos, J.J. (2013). Do young toddlers act on their social preferences? Developmental Psychology, 49, 1964-1970.
13.
Fantz, R.L. (1956). A method for studying early visual development. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 6, 13-15.
14.
Fantz, R.L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 146, 668-670.
15.
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 1-11.
16.
Griffiths, P.E., & Gray, R.D. (1994). Developmental systems and evolutionary explanation. The Journal of Philosophy, 91, 277-304.
17.
Griffiths, P.E., & Stotz, K. (2000). How the mind grows: A developmental perspective on the biology of cognition. Synthese, 122, 29-51.
18.
Haith, M.M. (1998). Who put the cog in infant cognition? Is rich interpretation too costly? Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 167-179.
19.
Haith, M.M., & Benson, J.B. (1998). Infant cognition. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief) & D. Kuhn & S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed., pp. 199-254). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
20.
Hamlin, J.K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: Evidence for an innate moral core. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 186-193.
21.
Hamlin, J.K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26, 30-39.
22.
Hamlin, J.K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13, 923-929.
23.
Keen, R. (2003). Representation of objects and events: Why do infants look so smart and toddlers look so dumb? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 79-83.
24.
Land, M.F., & McLeod, P. (2000). From eye movements to actions: How batsmen hit the ball. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1340-1345.
25.
Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2013). A developmental evolutionary framework for psychology. Review of General Psychology, 17, 184-189.
26.
Lewkowicz, D.J. (2011). The biological implausibility of the nature-nurture dichotomy and what it means for the study of infancy. Infancy, 16, 331-367.
27.
Luo, Y. (2011). Do 10-month-old infants understand others' false beliefs? Cognition, 121, 289-298.
28.
Machado, A., & Silva, F.J. (2007). Toward a richer view of the scientific method: The role of conceptual analysis. American Psychologist, 7, 671-681.
29.
Mameli, M., & Bateson, P. (2011). An evaluation of the concept of innateness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 366, 436-443.
30.
Onishi, K.H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 225-258.
31.
Overton, W.F. (2007). A coherent metatheory for dynamic systems: Relational organicism-contextualism. Human Development, 50, 154-159.
32.
Oyama, S. (2000). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution (2nd ed.). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
33.
Racine, T.P. (2012). Getting beyond rich and lean views of joint attention. In A. Seemann (Ed.), Joint attention: New developments in psychology, philosophy of mind, and social neuroscience (pp. 21-42). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
34.
Racine, T.P. (2013). How useful are the concepts ‘‘innate'' and ‘‘adaptation'' for explaining human development? Human Development, 56, 141-146.
35.
Scott, R., He, Z., Baillargeon, R., & Cummins, D. (2012). False-belief understanding in 2.5-year-olds: Evidence from two novel verbal spontaneous-response tasks. Developmental Science, 15, 181-193.
36.
Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief. Psychological Science, 18, 587-592.
37.
Spelke, E.S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55, 1233-1243.
38.
Spelke, E.S., & Kinzler, K.D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 89-96.
39.
Spencer, J.P., Blumber, M.S., McMurray, B., Robinson, S.R., Samuelson, L.K., & Tomblin, J.B. (2009). Short arms and talking eggs: Why we should no longer abide the nativist-empiricist debate. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 79-87.
40.
Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580-586.
41.
Tissaw, M.A. (2007). Making sense of neonatal imitation. Theory and Psychology, 17, 217-242.
42.
Wereha, T.J., & Racine, T.P. (2012). Evolution, development, and human social cognition. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3, 559-579.
43.
Witherington, D.C. (2007). The dynamic system approach as a metatheory for developmental psychology. Human Development, 50, 127-153.
44.
Witherington, D.C. (2011). Taking emergence seriously: The centrality of circular causality for dynamic systems approaches to development. Human Development, 54, 66-92.
Copyright / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Copyright: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be translated into other languages, reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, microcopying, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.
You do not currently have access to this content.