This brief review on perceived control and aging is organized according to 3 perspectives of research involving description, explanation, and modification. An extensive body of literature has utilized cross-sectional and correlational methods to describe the sociodemographic variations and outcomes associated with perceived control. This work has focused on differences in perceived control as a function of age, sex, education, socioeconomic status, and culture and has identified positive associations with many aging-related outcomes involving health and well-being. With growing evidence regarding the health benefits of perceived control in the context of a declining sense of control with aging, there has been an increased effort to uncover the mechanisms involved, with the hopes of developing methods to maintain and/or promote adaptive control beliefs throughout adulthood. Through longitudinal and experimental work, researchers are beginning to clarify the directionality and elucidate the mechanisms to explain the associations. Recent evidence from longitudinal studies shows that control beliefs have an impact on subsequent changes in health. Yet, the findings suggest that it is not a unidirectional relationship. A conceptual model suggesting an ongoing reciprocal relationship between perceived control and health and well-being is discussed. Research examining the mechanisms that link perceived control to aging-related outcomes can help to inform and to develop effective interventions that are tailored to the individual's specific barriers and goals. We consider new directions for research, including more attention to intraindividual variability and reactivity to daily challenges, such as stress, with the goal of advancing our understanding of how perceived control contributes to aging-related outcomes. More work is needed to develop strategies to enhance control beliefs in later life. Although it will not always be possible to modify control beliefs, researchers can take these beliefs into account when developing interventions. A personalized approach is recommended as a way to tailor interventions that are compatible with individuals' beliefs about control to facilitate adaptive behavior change. Conclusions focus on selected issues and considerations for future research.

1.
Rotter J: Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol Monogr Gen Appl 1966;80:1-28.
2.
Skinner EA: A guide to constructs of control. J Pers Soc Psychol 1996;71:549-570.
3.
Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Rickenbach EH: Making sense of control: change and consequences. Emerg Trends Soc Behav Sci 2015, DOI: 10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0209.
4.
Lachman M, Neupert SD, Agrigoroaei S: The relevance of control beliefs for health and aging; in Schaie KW, Willis SL (eds): Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. San Diego, CA, Elsevier, 2011, pp 175-190.
5.
Neupert SD, Allaire JC: I think I can, I think I can: examining the within-person coupling of control beliefs and cognition in older adults. Psychol Aging 2012;27:742-749.
6.
Langer EJ, Rodin J: The effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: a field experiment in an institutional setting. J Pers Soc Psychol 1976;34:191-198.
7.
Heckhausen J, Wrosch C, Schulz R: A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychol Rev 2010;117:1-53.
8.
Mirowsky J, Ross CE: Life course trajectories of perceived control and their relationship to education. Am J Sociol 2007;112:1339-1382.
9.
Lachman ME, Rosnick CB, Röcke C: The rise and fall of control beliefs and life satisfaction in adulthood: trajectories of stability and change over ten years. Aging Cogn Res Methodol Empir Adv 2009:143-160.
10.
Infurna F, Okun M: Antecedents and outcomes of level and rates of change in perceived control: the moderating role of age. Dev Psychol 2015;51:1420-1437.
11.
Drewelies J, Wagner J, Tesch-Römer C, et al: Perceived control across the second half of life: the role of physical health and social integration. Psychol Aging 2017;32:76-92.
12.
Lachman M: Perceived control over aging-related declines: adaptive beliefs and behaviors. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2006;15:282-286.
13.
Barlow M, Wrosch C, Heckhausen J, et al: Control strategies for managing physical health problems in old age: evidence for the motivational theory of life-span development; in Reich JW, Infurna FJ (eds): Perceived Control: Theory, Research, and Practice in the First 50 Years. New York, NY, Oxford University Press, 2016, pp 281-307.
14.
Heckhausen J, Schulz R: A life-span theory of control. Psychol Rev 1995;102:284-304.
15.
Wrosch C, Heckhausen J, Lachman ME: Goal management across adulthood and old age: the adaptive value of primary and secondary control; in Mroczek DK, Little TD (eds): Handbook of Personality Development. Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 2006, pp 399-421.
16.
Lachman ME, Weaver SL: The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;74:763-773.
17.
Wolinsky FD, Stump TE: Age and the sense of control among older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1996;51:S217-S220.
18.
Turiano NA, Chapman BP, Agrigoroaei S, et al: Perceived control reduces mortality risk at low, not high, education levels. Health Psychol 2014;33:883-890.
19.
Vargas Lascano DI, Galambos NL, Krahn HJ, et al: Growth in perceived control across 25 years from the late teens to midlife: the role of personal and parents' education. Dev Psychol 2015;51:124-135.
20.
Kitayama S, Karasawa M, Curhan KB, et al: Independence and interdependence predict health and wellbeing: divergent patterns in the United States and Japan. Front Psychol 2010;1:1-10.
21.
Kunzmann U, Little T, Smith J: Perceiving control: a double-edged sword in old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2002;57:484-491.
22.
Lachman M, Röcke C, Rosnick C, et al: Realism and illusion in Americans' temporal views of their life satisfaction: age differences in reconstructing the past and anticipating the future. Psychol Sci 2008;19:889-197.
23.
Infurna FJ, Ram N, Gerstorf D: Level and change in perceived control predict 19-year mortality: findings from the Americans' Changing Lives Study. Dev Psychol 2013;49:1833-1847.
24.
Infurna FJ, Gerstorf D: Linking perceived control, physical activity, and biological health to memory change. Psychol Aging 2013;28:1147-1163.
25.
Lachman ME, Andreoletti C: Strategy use mediates the relationship between control beliefs and memory performance for middle-aged and older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2006;61:88-94.
26.
Rebok G, Rasmusson DX, Brandt J: Prospects for computerized memory training in normal elderly: effects of practice on explicit and implicit memory tasks. Appl Cogn Psychol 1996;10:211-223.
27.
Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Martin R, et al: Does cognitive training improve internal locus of control among older adults? J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2010;65:591-598.
28.
Infurna FJ, Gerstorf D, Zarit SH: Examining dynamic links between perceived control and health: longitudinal evidence for differential effects in midlife and old age. Dev Psychol 2011;47:9-18.
29.
Gerstorf D, Röcke C, Lachman ME: Antecedent-consequent relations of perceived control to health and social support : longitudinal evidence for between-domain associations across adulthood. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2011;66:61-71.
30.
Soederberg Miller L, Lachman ME: The sense of control and cognitive aging. Soc Cogn Aging 1999:17-41.
31.
Bandura A: Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY, WH Freeman & Co, 1997.
32.
Uchino BN: Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med 2006;29:377-387.
33.
Lang FR, Heckhausen J: Perceived control over development and subjective well-being: differential benefits across adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol 2001;81:509-523.
34.
Windsor TD, Anstey KJ: Age differences in psychosocial predictors of positive and negative affect: a longitudinal investigation of young, midlife, and older adults. Psychol Aging 2010;25:641-652.
35.
Pressman SD, Cohen S: Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull 2005;131:925-971.
36.
White S, Wójcicki T, McAuley E: Social cognitive influences on physical activity behavior in middle-aged and older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2012;67:18-26.
37.
Lachman M, Firth K: The adaptive value of feeling in control during midlife; in Brim OG, Ryff CD, Kessler RC (eds): How Healthy Are We? A National Study of Well-Being at Midlife. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 2004, pp 320-349.
38.
Antonucci TC: Social relations: an examination of social networks, social support, and sense of control; in Birren J, Schaie KW (eds): Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. San Diego, CA, Academic Press, 2001, pp 427-453.
39.
Diehl M, Hay EL: Risk and resilience factors in coping with daily stress in adulthood: the role of age, self-concept incoherence, and personal control. Dev Psychol 2010;46:1132-1146.
40.
Zautra AJ, Davis MC, Reich JW, et al: Phone-based interventions with automated mindfulness and mastery messages improve the daily functioning for depressed middle-aged community residents. J Psychother Integr 2012;22:206-228.
41.
Lachman ME, Weaver SL, Bandura M, et al: Improving memory and control beliefs through cognitive restructuring and self-generated strategies. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 1992;47:293-299.
42.
Neupert S, Lachman M, Whitbourne S: Exercise self-efficacy and control beliefs predict exercise behavior after an exercise intervention for older adults. J Aging Phys Act 2009;17:1-14.
43.
Parisi JM, Gross AL, Marsiske M, et al: Control beliefs and cognition over a 10-year period: findings from the ACTIVE trial. Psychol Aging 2017;32:69-75.
44.
West RL, Bagwell DK, Dark-Freudeman A: Self-efficacy and memory aging: the impact of a memory intervention based on self-efficacy. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2008;15:302-329.
45.
Payne BR, Jackson JJ, Hill PL, et al: Memory self-efficacy predicts responsiveness to inductive reasoning training in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2012;67:27-35.
46.
Tennstedt S, Howland J, Lachman M, et al: A randomized, controlled trial of a group intervention to reduce fear of falling and associated activity restriction in older adults. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 1998;53:384-392.
47.
Eizenman DR, Nesselroade JR, Featherman DL, et al: Intraindividual variability in perceived control in an older sample: the MacArthur successful aging studies. Psychol Aging 1997;12:489-502.
48.
Agrigoroaei S, Neupert SD, Lachman ME: Maintaining a sense of control in the context of cognitive challenge: greater stability in control beliefs benefits working memory. GeroPsych (Bern) 2013;26:45-49.
49.
Almeida DM: Resilience and vulnerability to daily stressors assessed via diary methods. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2005;14:64-68.
50.
Infurna FJ, Luthar SS: Resilience to major life stressors is not as common as thought. Perspect Psychol Sci 2015;11:175-194.
51.
Neupert SD, Almeida DM, Charles ST: Age differences in reactivity to daily stressors: the role of personal control. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2007;62:P216-P225.
52.
Infurna FJ, Reich JW: Perceived control: 50 years of innovation and another 50 to go; in Reich JW, Infurna FJ (eds): Perceived Control: Theory, Research, and Practice in the First 50 Years. Oxford, Oxford University Press, 2016, pp 1-22.
53.
Bierut LJ, Johnson EO, Saccone NL: A glimpse into the future - personalized medicine for smoking cessation. Neuropharmacology 2014;76:592-599.
54.
Clark M, Hampson SE, Avery L, et al: Effects of a tailored lifestyle self-management intervention in patients with type 2 diabetes. Br J Health Psychol 2004;9:365-379.
55.
Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, et al: Internet-based personalized feedback to reduce 21st-birthday drinking: a randomized controlled trial of an event-specific prevention intervention. J Consult Clin Psychol 2009;77:51-63.
56.
Agrigoroaei S, Polito M, Lee A, et al: Cortisol response to challenge involving low controllability: the role of control beliefs and age. Biol Psychol 2013;93:138-142.
57.
Evans G, Shapiro DH, Lewis MA: Specifying dysfunctional mismatches between different control dimensions. Br J Psychol 1993;84:255-273.
58.
Baltes M: Dependency in old age: gains and losses. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 1995;4:14-19.
59.
Weiner B: An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychol Rev 1985;92:548-573.
60.
Chipperfield JG, Perry RP, Pekrun R, et al: The paradoxical role of perceived control in late life health behavior. PLoS One 2016;11:1-16.
Copyright / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Copyright: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be translated into other languages, reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, microcopying, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.
You do not currently have access to this content.