Lemur catta has traditionally been considered a species with male-biased dispersal; however, occasional female dispersal occurs. Using molecular data, we evaluated dispersal patterns in 2 L. catta populations in southwestern Madagascar: Tsimanampesotse National Park (TNP) and Bezà Mahafaly Special Reserve (BMSR). We also investigated the genetic differentiation between the populations and dispersal partner relatedness. Results showed minor genetic differentiation between the populations (ϴST = 0.039), which may indicate gene flow historically occurring in this region, made possible by the presence of L. catta groups between the sites. Different patterns of sex-biased dispersal were found between the sites using corrected assignment indices: male-biased dispersal in TNP, and a lack of sex-biased dispersal in BMSR. Observational evidence of female dispersal in BMSR supports these results and may imply intense female resource competition in and around BMSR, because small groups of 2-3 females have been observed dispersing within BMSR and entering the reserve from outside. These dispersing groups largely consisted of mothers transferring with daughters, although we have an aunt-niece pair transferring together. Genetic data suggest that males also transfer with relatives. Our data demonstrate that dispersal partners consist of same-sexed kin for L. catta males and females, highlighting the importance of kin selection.

Whether or not sex-biased dispersal is found in a population depends on how resource competition affects each of the sexes. Generally, female philopatry occurs where females can defend high-quality resources with relatives [Wrangham, 1980]. Intrasexual competition for mates [Jack, 2003] and/or inbreeding avoidance [Pusey, 1987] often drives males to disperse in such species. Conversely, where females disperse, they often do so due to local resource competition [Greenwood, 1980].

In this study, we set out to evaluate dispersal patterns in 2 wild populations of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) [Sauther et al., 1999; Jolly et al., 2006] by using a combination of observational and genetic data. Genetic methods are increasingly being used to study dispersal [Di Fiore, 2003]. When paired with observational data, molecular data can help provide a more nuanced understanding of a species' social structure and behavior [Lawler et al., 2003; Bradley et al., 2004; Di Fiore et al., 2009; Harris et al., 2009; Baden et al., 2014].

Historically, L. catta males have been viewed as the sex that disperses, and they usually transfer between groups in pairs or triplets rather than alone [Jones, 1983; Sussman, 1991, 1992; Gould, 1994, 1997; Koyama et al., 2002; Gould, 2006; Kelley, 2013]. Observations from at least 2 research sites, however, have suggested that female dispersal can also occur in L. catta [Sauther et al., 1999; Koyama et al., 2002]. These observations pose the question of whether evidence for female dispersal will be revealed by molecular data. Results from this evaluation can potentially indicate the level of resource competition facing females in different L. catta habitats. As part of our molecular evaluation of dispersal patterns, we also sought to determine the level of genetic differentiation between our study sites. Low differentiation, if found, would indicate that the 2 locations may have historically experienced gene flow. The degree of relatedness of transfer partners was also determined with molecular data. Such information can be helpful in indicating the importance of kin selection in the dispersal of this species or whether individuals travel between groups with unrelated individuals. No study has previously evaluated the level of genetic differentiation between wild L. catta populations, used genetics to test for sex biases in dispersal in this species or measured the degree of relatedness between dispersing individuals using genetics.

Study Sites and Sample Collection

We studied L. catta in 2 areas of southwestern Madagascar: Bezà Mahafaly Special Reserve (BMSR) and Tsimanampesotse National Park (TNP) [Parga et al., 2012]. TNP is located approximately 135 km southwest of BMSR [Cuozzo et al., 2008]. L. catta groups are present in the region between TNP and BMSR [Sauther, pers. observation]. Although there is an absence of major geographic barriers to dispersal (i.e. rivers [Guschanski et al., 2007; Quéméré et al., 2010]) between the 2 locations, potential anthropogenic barriers to dispersal exist, such as deforested patches [Brinkmann et al., 2014].

Hair and blood samples were collected from BMSR (n = 243) between the years of 1987 and 2006 as part of a long-term monitoring of this population [Sussman et al., 2012]. Blood samples were collected at TNP (n = 25) in 2006. Individuals were captured using a Telinject blow dart system and were administered a drug mixture of ketamine and/or ketamine and diazepam, based on protocols developed over the past 25 years and 360 captures [Sauther et al., 2006]. All animal handling was conducted with Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approval from the University of Colorado and/or the University of North Dakota. Regular monthly censuses began in BMSR in the latter half of 2005. Prior to this, instances of dispersal were documented yearly. Dispersal events involving pairs or small groups of individuals in BMSR were included in this study only if genetic information was available for one or more of the dispersing individuals. A dispersal event was noted when an individual entered a new group for at least 6 months.

Genetic Analyses

A standard phenol-chloroform extraction [Sambrook et al., 1989] was used to obtain genomic DNA from hair and blood samples. For blood samples collected on Schleicher & Schuell IsoCode cards, PCR amplification of microsatellites was performed as described in Parga et al. [2012]. Both species-specific and heterologous microsatellites were used: Lc5, Lc6, Lc7, Lc8, Lc9, Lc10 [Pastorini et al., 2005], 69HDZ267, 69HDZ299 [Zaonarivelo et al., 2007], Efr09 [Jekielek and Strobeck, 1999], Efr02 [Wimmer, 2000], L-2 [Merenlender, 1993], Em7 [Pastorini et al., 2004], Em12 [Pastorini, this study; forward: gaacctgggtggctacattc, reverse: gtttgtattaggcttggctgc], and Pv1 [Lawler et al., 2001]. Approximately 10-100 ng template DNA was amplified in 12.5- or 20-µl reactions (see Pastorini et al. [2005] and Parga et al. [2012]). A total of 243 samples were analyzed from BMSR, gathered across 6 collection years and 11 different groups between 1987 and 2006. From TNP, 25 samples from 4 different groups were analyzed from a single year, 2006.

MICRO-CHECKER version 2.2.3 [van Oosterhout et al., 2004] was used to evaluate the data for null alleles and scoring errors. One microsatellite (Lc9) showed evidence of scoring errors and null alleles, so was discarded. POPGENE version 1.31 [Yeh et al., 1999] was used to test each locus for Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium within each population. Loci not in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (table 1) were excluded from the calculation of ϴST, tests of population differentiation, FIS and tests of sex-biased dispersal.

Table 1

Heterozygosity and allele number for each locus

Heterozygosity and allele number for each locus
Heterozygosity and allele number for each locus

FSTAT version 2.9.3 [Goudet, 2001] was used to calculate ϴST and FIS following Weir and Cockerham [1984]. The test for population differentiation in FSTAT uses the log likelihood statistic G [Goudet et al., 1996] and was run using 1,000 permutations, testing the value for ϴST against the null hypothesis of an absence of population differentiation (ϴST = 0). Only samples collected in 2006 were used to test for population differentiation and to generate FIS values, as that was the single year in which samples were available from both TNP and BMSR.

Assignment indices (AI) [Paetkau et al., 1995] use an individual's multilocus genotype to identify the likelihood that the individual was born into a particular population. We used GenAlEx 6.5 [Peakall and Smouse, 2012] to calculate the corrected AI (AIc), for use in sex-biased dispersal tests. AIc values were calculated by subtracting the population mean AI as calculated per analyzed year from each individual's AI, with positive AIc values indicating that an individual is likely philopatric (resident to the area), while strongly negative values indicate rare genotypes and likely immigrant status [Favre et al., 1997; Mossman and Waser, 1999]. Tests for sex-biased dispersal were run separately within each population, and were run for BMSR samples per collection year. Only years in which genotypes were available for ≥20 individuals (males plus females) were included in sex-biased dispersal tests. Mann-Whitney U tests in GenAlEx were used to determine if there was a significant difference in the AIc values of males versus females in each population.

ML-Relate was used to estimate pairwise relatedness using maximum likelihood methods [Kalinowski et al., 2006]. The putative relationship (parent-offspring, full-sibling, half-sibling, unrelated) identified as most likely was tested against the second most likely alternative using likelihood ratio tests, with a 0.05 significance level. Overall within-sex relatedness estimates were also calculated per site.

In some cases, CERVUS 3.0.3 [Marshall et al., 1998; Kalinowski et al., 2007] was used to help clarify the relationship between individuals transferring together via the identification of those individuals' parents [for details, see Lawler et al., 2003].

For all microsatellites used, heterozygosity ranged between 0.588 and 0.900 (table 1). Genetic differentiation between the 2 populations, BMSR and TNP, was low (ϴST = 0.039) but significantly different from 0 (p < 0.001), as calculated across all loci. When calculated separately for each sex, significance was again reached (p < 0.001), with females (ϴST = 0.056) showing greater genetic differentiation than males (ϴST = 0.035).

The mean FIS (averaged across all loci) was positive for both populations (BMSR: 0.024; TNP: 0.018). When calculated per sex, BMSR males showed a positive mean FIS (0.047), whereas females showed a negative mean FIS (-0.004). At TNP, both males and females showed positive mean values of FIS (0.019 and 0.102, respectively).

Overall variance in AIc was greater for males than for females in each year and in both sites; however, when comparing individual male versus female AIc values per site and sex to assess dispersal trends, only TNP showed significantly more negative scores among males than females (fig. 1; table 2). At BMSR during the same year (2006), males and females showed an extensive overlap in AIc values, and no significant sex bias in dispersal was detected (fig. 1; table 2). This lack of sex bias in dispersal in BMSR held consistently for all 5 study years tested (table 2).

Table 2

Variance in corrected assignment indices (vAIc) and mean assignment indices (mAIc) per site and per year, with Mann-Whitney U results for sex-biased dispersal tests

Variance in corrected assignment indices (vAIc) and mean assignment indices (mAIc) per site and per year, with Mann-Whitney U results for sex-biased dispersal tests
Variance in corrected assignment indices (vAIc) and mean assignment indices (mAIc) per site and per year, with Mann-Whitney U results for sex-biased dispersal tests
Fig. 1

The range of AIc scores in 2006 per sex and per site, representing each individual as a horizontal line. Note that TNP shows a significant male bias in dispersal (p = 0.002), while BMSR shows no sex bias in dispersal (p = 0.57; table 2). Positive values are associated with population residents, while negative scores indicate rare genotypes more likely to be associated with immigrants. Filled squares indicate mean AIc values.

Fig. 1

The range of AIc scores in 2006 per sex and per site, representing each individual as a horizontal line. Note that TNP shows a significant male bias in dispersal (p = 0.002), while BMSR shows no sex bias in dispersal (p = 0.57; table 2). Positive values are associated with population residents, while negative scores indicate rare genotypes more likely to be associated with immigrants. Filled squares indicate mean AIc values.

Close modal

Genetic data were available for 5 dispersal events by pairs or small groups of individuals at BMSR (table 3). In the first event, 2 adult and 1 subadult female immigrated as a small group into BMSR from outside of the reserve. Genetic data revealed that these 2 adults were most likely a mother-daughter pair (no genetic information was available for the subadult female). The conditions under which these 3 females left their original group were unknown, but they were in poor physical condition and injured upon entry into BMSR. The group they transferred into had recently lost half of their reproductive females (2 of 4). Within several months of transferring into this group, another original female was killed by a dog and by the end of the next year the remaining original female disappeared, leaving only the immigrant females. In all other instances, females transferred between groups within BMSR, and were known to have dispersed because they were forcibly evicted from their social groups. Event 2 was a case of female dispersal with offspring (a daughter and son; table 3). Our data also show that 2 females dispersing between groups together (event 3, table 3) shared an aunt-niece relationship. The mothers of these 2 females were determined to have a parent-offspring relationship via CERVUS and were no longer present in the group at the time of the joint dispersal of their daughters, although both dispersing females had other female relatives present in the group (i.e. sister, aunts) at the time of their departure.

Table 3

Dispersal events by pairs or small groups in BMSR for which genetic data were available

Dispersal events by pairs or small groups in BMSR for which genetic data were available
Dispersal events by pairs or small groups in BMSR for which genetic data were available

For males, dispersal partners were also related. In event 4 (table 3), 2 adult males transferring between 2 nonnatal groups appeared to be related at the level of half-siblings. In event 5, a pair of same-aged males dispersing from their natal group together were distantly related enough to return an r value of 0 (table 3). CERVUS, however, was used to determine these males' mothers, who were related at the level of first cousins - making the same-aged dispersing natal males related at the level of second cousins.

Although it was unknown where some groups of dispersing individuals immigrated to or originated from (e.g. event 1, table 3), dispersing pairs/groups of females generally left groups because they were aggressively evicted by other female residents (events 2 and 3, table 3), and either joined other established groups (event 1, table 3) or formed the basis of a new group (event 2, table 3). In contrast, dispersing males left groups of their own volition to create new groups or enter other fully formed groups.

A comparison of relatedness values of dispersal partners in table 3 with estimates of average relatedness calculated within each sex at each site (BMSR: males, r = 0.074, females, r = 0.071 ; TNP: males, r = 0.0497, females, r = 0.067) shows that on average, dispersal partners were more related than were random individuals of the same sex in the population. A consideration of average relatedness within each sex also shows that only in TNP was the average female relatedness greater than male relatedness, as would be expected in a species with female philopatry and male dispersal. Conversely, BMSR males and females showed nearly the same average relatedness, which would be consistent with both male and female dispersal occurring in BMSR.

L. catta at BMSR and TNP showed only minor genetic differentiation (as measured by ϴST), but the difference was significant. The area between the 2 habitats contains mixed dry scrub and dry forest, and there are L. catta groups present along the road between Ambatry and TNP [Sauther, Cuozzo, pers. observation]. Given sufficient mobility and interbreeding with the L. catta groups living in the area between BMSR and TNP, the 2 populations may have historically experienced gene flow, which could have resulted in our finding of low genetic differentiation between the sites.

Repeating tests for genetic differentiation (ϴST) within each sex separately showed that greater differentiation existed among females than among males, as would be expected where males are the dispersing sex [Favre et al., 1997]. The variance in AIc values was also consistently higher for males than for females across all years and in both sites, which is the pattern expected with male-biased dispersal [Goudet et al., 2002]. These results suggest that gene flow among females has been more restricted than gene flow among males, which is in agreement with behavioral data on intergroup movement showing male-biased dispersal in L. catta (BMSR [Sussman, 1991, 1992; Gould, 1994, 1997, 2006]; Berenty, Madagascar [Jones, 1983; Koyama et al., 2002]).

The mean FIS was slightly positive for both populations. Rather than indicating inbreeding, this positive mean FIS likely resulted from the sampling of structured populations consisting of distinct social groups having nonrandom mating [Sugg et al., 1996]. When calculated per sex in each site, only males in BMSR showed the expected result of the dispersing sex having a mean FIS value that is positive and higher than that of females [Lawson Handley and Perrin, 2007].

Tests to detect sex-biased dispersal comparing individual corrected assignment indices between males and females per year revealed evidence for male-biased dispersal at TNP, but not at BMSR. There was no significant difference between males and females in corrected assignment indices for all of the study years tested at BMSR, suggesting that female dispersal occurs in addition to male dispersal at this site. Observational data on pairs and small groups of females entering BMSR also support this result [Sauther et al., 1999; this study].

In sum, although there are several indicators of male-biased dispersal in both sites, the AIc sex-biased dispersal test results, within-sex relatedness measures at each site, and observational evidence support female dispersal in BMSR. Why female dispersal appears to occur in BMSR but not TNP is unclear. Because 2 instances of observed female dispersal in BMSR were driven by female eviction, it is possible that the factors which lead to increased competition among females and group fission [Koyama et al., 2002] may be the primary driver of female dispersal in L. catta. Indeed, in the single case of female dispersal in BMSR where group sex ratio was known for pre- and posttransfer groups, the dispersing females transferred into a group with fewer females than were in their original group (8 and 2, respectively). In addition, for the case of the 2 females and a subadult transferring from outside of BMSR, the females joined a group that had recently lost half of their adult females and within a year they were the only females remaining in the group. Interestingly, female dispersal in L. catta has also been documented in at least 1 other wild research site (Berenty [Koyama et al., 2002]). To reach any solid conclusions about why female dispersal appears to happen in some, but not all, populations of this species, further research is needed on how variables related to resource competition, such as habitat quality or population density, might be affecting dispersal patterns.

Although based on a small sample size, our genetic data show that individuals of both sexes disperse with kin who are markedly more related to them than other random same-sex individuals in the population. Among 3 groups of dispersing females, genetic data reveal a parent-offspring bond in 2 cases, and in a third, a likely aunt-niece relationship. One pair of adult males transferring between groups together was related at the level of half-siblings. Parallel dispersal [Schoof et al., 2009] of relatives has been found in a few other primates, e.g. vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) [Cheney and Seyfarth, 1983] and white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) [Jack and Fedigan, 2004]. This strategy can have many advantages. In addition to inclusive fitness benefits, practicing parallel dispersal with a relative can provide several direct benefits, such as partnership in affiliative interactions and aiding in the detection of predators or aggressive conspecifics during the transfer process [Sussman, 1992; Gould, 1994, 1997, 2006]. Hence, dispersing with kin can offer fitness gains, and this strategy appears to be used by both male and female L. catta. One interesting avenue for future research would be to assess the extent to which individuals disperse with relatives at other L. catta study sites, and to evaluate whether males ever transfer between groups with their male offspring.

We thank guest editor Teague O'Mara and 2 anonymous reviewers for helpful comments that improved the paper. We thank Don Melnick (Columbia University), Michael Krützen (University of Zürich), Nick Mundy and Bill Amos (University of Cambridge) for access to their laboratory facilities and for valuable assistance during the laboratory work. We thank Jean Dubach (Loyola University) for sharing information on microsatellite primer optimization conditions. M.L.S. and F.P.C. thank all of the field assistants and veterinary personnel acknowledged in our previous publications who aided data collection from 2003 through 2012 at BMSR and TNP. Funding was provided to J.A.P. by the National Science Foundation (Minority Postdoctoral Fellowship), the Canada Foundation for Innovation, Ontario Research Fund, the University of Toronto and California State University-Los Angeles. Research by M.L.S. and F.P.C. was supported by the University of Colorado, the University of North Dakota, the St. Louis Zoo (FRC 06-1), the National Geographic Society, the International Primatological Society, the American Society of Primatologists, Primate Conservation Inc., ND EPSCoR and the National Science Foundation, BCS 0922465. L.G. was funded by NSERC, the Wenner-Gren Foundation and the National Geographic Society. R.R.L. was supported by funding from the National Science Foundation (DEB-9902146). J.P. acknowledges financial support from the Swiss National Science Foundation, Vontobel Foundation, Julius Klaus Foundation, A.H. Schultz Foundation and the Cleveland Zoological Society.

1.
Baden AL, Holmes SM, Johnson SE, Engberg SE, Louis EE Jr, Bradley BJ (2014). Species-level view of population structure and gene flow for a critically endangered primate (Varecia variegata). Ecology and Evolution 4: 2675-2692.
2.
Bradley BJ, Doran-Sheehy DM, Lukas D, Boesch C, Vigilant L (2004). Dispersed male networks in western gorillas. Current Biology 14: 510-513.
3.
Brinkmann K, Noromiarilanto F, Ratovonamana RY, Buerkert A (2014). Deforestation processes in south-western Madagascar over the past 40 years: what can we learn from settlement characteristics? Agriculture, Ecosystems and Environment 195: 231-243.
4.
Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1983). Nonrandom dispersal in free-ranging vervet monkeys: social and genetic consequences. American Naturalist 122: 392-412.
5.
Cuozzo FP, Sauther ML, Yamashita N, Lawler RR, Brockman DK, Godfrey LR, Gould L, Youssouf IAJ, Lent C, Ratsirarson J, Richard AF, Scott JR, Sussman RW, Villers LM, Weber MA, Willis G (2008). A comparison of salivary pH in sympatric wild lemurs (Lemur catta and Propithecus verreauxi) at Beza Mahafaly Special Reserve, Madagascar. American Journal of Primatology 70: 363-371.
6.
Di Fiore A (2003). Molecular genetic approaches to the study of primate behavior, social organization, and reproduction. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 46: 62-99.
7.
Di Fiore A, Link A, Schmitt CA, Spehar SN (2009). Dispersal patterns in sympatric woolly and spider monkeys: integrating molecular and observational data. Behaviour 146: 437-470.
8.
Favre L, Balloux F, Goudet J, Perrin N (1997). Female-biased dispersal in the monogamous mammal Crocidura russula: evidence from field data and microsatellite patterns. Proceedings of the Royal Society (London) B 264: 127-132.
9.
Goudet J (2001). FSTAT, a program to estimate and test gene diversities and fixation indices (version 2.9.3). http://www2.unil.ch/popgen/softwares/fstat.htm.
10.
Goudet J, Perrin N, Waser P (2002). Tests for sex-biased dispersal using bi-parentally inherited genetic markers. Molecular Ecology 11: 1103-1114.
11.
Goudet J, Raymond M, de Meeus T, Rousset F (1996). Testing differentiation in diploid populations. Genetics 144: 1933-1940.
12.
Gould L (1994). Patterns of Affiliative Behavior in Adult Male Ringtailed Lemurs (Lemur catta) at the Beza-Mahafaly Reserve, Madagascar. PhD dissertation, Washington University, St Louis.
13.
Gould L (1997). Intermale affiliative behavior in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at the Beza-Mahafaly Reserve, Madagascar. Primates 38: 15-30.
14.
Gould L (2006). Male sociality and integration during the dispersal process in Lemur catta: a case study. In Ringtailed Lemur Biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar (Jolly A, Sussman RW, Koyama N, Rasamimanana H, eds.), pp 296-310. New York, Springer.
15.
Greenwood PJ (1980). Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals. Animal Behaviour 28: 1140-1162.
16.
Guschanski K, Olivieri G, Funk SM, Radespiel U (2007). MtDNA reveals strong genetic differentiation among geographically isolated populations of the golden brown mouse lemur, Microcebus ravelobensis. Conservation Genetics 8: 809-821.
17.
Harris TR, Caillaud D, Chapman CA, Vigilant L (2009). Neither genetic nor observational data alone are sufficient for understanding sex-biased dispersal in a social-group-living species. Molecular Ecology 18: 1777-1790.
18.
Jack K (2003). Males on the move: evolutionary explanations of secondary dispersal by male primates. Primate Report 67: 61-83.
19.
Jack KM, Fedigan L (2004). Male dispersal patterns in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. 1. Patterns and causes of natal emigration. Animal Behaviour 67: 761-769.
20.
Jekielek J, Strobeck C (1999). Characterization of polymorphic brown lemur (Eulemur fulvus) microsatellite loci and their amplification in the family Lemuridae. Molecular Ecology 8: 901-903.
21.
Jolly A, Sussman RW, Koyama N, Rasamimanana H (eds.) (2006). Ringtailed Lemur Biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar. New York, Springer.
22.
Jones KC (1983). Inter-troop transfer of Lemur catta males at Berenty, Madagascar. Folia Primatologica 40: 145-160.
23.
Kalinowski ST, Taper ML, Marshall TC (2007). Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment. Molecular Ecology 16: 1099-1106.
24.
Kalinowski ST, Wagner AP, Taper ML (2006). ML-RELATE: a computer program for maximum likelihood estimation of relatedness and relationship. Molecular Ecology Notes 6: 576-579.
25.
Kelley EA (2013). The ranging behavior of Lemur catta in the region of Cap Sainte-Marie, Madagascar. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 150: 122-132.
26.
Koyama N, Nakamichi M, Ichino S, Takahata Y (2002). Population and social dynamics changes in ring-tailed lemur troops at Berenty, Madagascar between 1989-1999. Primates 43: 291-314.
27.
Lawler RR, Richard AF, Riley MA (2001). Characterization and screening of microsatellite loci in a wild lemur population (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi). American Journal of Primatology 55: 253-259.
28.
Lawler RR, Richard AF, Riley MA (2003). Genetic population structure of the white sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi) at Beza Mahafaly Special Reserve, southwest Madagascar (1992-2001). Molecular Ecology 12: 2307-2317.
29.
Lawson Handley LJ, Perrin N (2007). Advances in our understanding of mammalian sex-biased dispersal. Molecular Ecology 16: 1559-1578.
30.
Marshall TC, Slate J, Kruuk LEB, Pemberton JM (1998). Statistical confidence for likelihood-based paternity inference in natural populations. Molecular Ecology 7: 639-655.
31.
Merenlender AM (1993). The Effects of Sociality on the Demography and Genetic Structure of Lemur fulvus rufus (Polygamous) and Lemur rubriventer (Monogamous) and the Conservation Implications. PhD dissertation, University of Rochester, New York.
32.
Mossman CA, Waser PM (1999). Genetic detection of sex-biased dispersal. Molecular Ecology 8: 1063-1067.
33.
Nei M (1978). Estimation of average heterozygosity and genetic distance from a small number of individuals. Genetics 89: 583-590.
34.
Paetkau D, Calvert W, Stirling I, Strobeck C (1995). Microsatellite analysis of population structure in Canadian polar bears. Molecular Ecology 4: 347-354.
35.
Parga JA, Sauther ML, Cuozzo FP, Youssouf Jacky IA, Lawler RR (2012). Evaluating ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) from southwestern Madagascar for a genetic population bottleneck. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 147: 21-29.
36.
Pastorini J, Fernando P, Forstner MRJ, Melnick DJ (2005). Characterization of new microsatellite loci for the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Molecular Ecology Notes 5: 149-151.
37.
Pastorini J, Fernando P, Melnick DJ, Forstner MRJ (2004). Isolation of 10 microsatellite markers for mongoose lemurs (Eulemur mongoz). Molecular Ecology Notes 4: 67-69.
38.
Peakall R, Smouse PE (2012). GenAlEx 6.5: genetic analysis in Excel. Population genetic software for teaching and research - an update. Bioinformatics 28: 2537-2539.
39.
Pusey AE (1987). Sex-biased dispersal and inbreeding avoidance in birds and mammals. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 2: 295-299.
40.
Quéméré E, Crouau-Roy B, Rabarivola C, Louis EE Jr, Chikhi L (2010). Landscape genetics of an endangered lemur (Propithecus tattersalli) within its entire fragmented range. Molecular Ecology 19: 1606-1621.
41.
Sambrook J, Fritsch EF, Maniatis T (1989). Molecular Cloning: A Laboratory Manual, 2nd ed. Cold Spring Harbor, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.
42.
Sauther ML, Sussman RW, Gould L (1999). The socioecology of the ringtailed lemur: thirty-five years of research. Evolutionary Anthropology 8: 120-132.
43.
Sauther ML, Fish KD, Cuozzo FP, Miller DS, Hunter-Ishikawa M, Culbertson H (2006). Patterns of health, disease, and behavior among wild ring-tailed lemurs, Lemur catta: effects of habitat and sex. In Ringtailed Lemur Biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar (Jolly A, Sussman RW, Koyama N, Rasamimanana H, eds.), pp 313-331. New York, Springer Science & Business Media.
44.
Schoof VAM, Jack KM, Isbell LA (2009). What traits promote male parallel dispersal in primates? Behaviour 146: 701-726.
45.
Sugg DW, Chesser RK, Dobson FS, Hoogland JL (1996). Population genetics meets behavioral ecology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 11: 338-342.
46.
Sussman RW (1991). Demography and social organization of free-ranging Lemur catta in the Beza Mahafaly Reserve, Madagascar. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 84: 43-58.
47.
Sussman RW (1992). Male life history and intergroup mobility among ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). International Journal of Primatology 13: 395-413.
48.
Sussman RW, Richard AF, Ratsirarson J, Sauther ML, Brockman DK, Gould L, Lawler R, Cuozzo FP (2012). Beza Mahafaly Special Reserve: long-term research on lemurs in southwestern Madagascar. In Long-Term Field Studies of Primates (Kappeler PM, Watts DP, eds.), pp 45-66. Berlin, Springer.
49.
Van Oosterhout C, Hutchinson WF, Wills DPM, Shipley P (2004). MICRO-CHECKER: software for identifying and correcting genotyping errors in microsatellite data. Molecular Ecology Notes 4: 535-538.
50.
Weir BS, Cockerham CC (1984). Estimating F-statistics for the analysis of population structure. Evolution 38: 1358-1370.
51.
Wimmer B (2000). Untersuchung der Paarungssysteme und der Populationsstruktur von Lemuren an Coquerels Zwergmaki (Mirza coquereli), dem grauen Mauslemur (Microcebus murinus), dem Rotstirnmaki (Eulemur fulvus rufus) und dem Larvensifaka (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi). PhD thesis, Universität München.
52.
Wrangham RW (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75: 262-300.
53.
Yeh FC, Yang R-C, Boyle TB, Ye Z-H, Mao JX (1999). POPGENE, user-friendly shareware for population genetic analysis. Edmonton, Molecular Biology and Biotechnology Centre, University of Alberta.
54.
Zaonarivelo JR, Andriantompohavana R, Shore GE, Engberg SE, McGuire SM, Louis EE Jr, Brenneman RA (2007). Characterization of 21 microsatellite marker loci in the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Conservation Genetics 8: 1209-1212.
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes as well as any distribution of modified material requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.