The ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta) is a group-living strepsirrhine primate endemic to Madagascar that faces considerable predation pressure from aerial and terrestrial predators. This species engages in mobbing and vigilance behavior in response to predators, and has referential alarm vocalizations. Because L. catta is female dominant, less is known about the alarm calls of males. We tested 3 hypotheses for male antipredator vocalization behavior on L. catta at the Bezà Mahafaly Special Reserve in Madagascar: the predator confusion, group maintenance, and predation risk allocation hypotheses. We found support for 2 hypotheses. When a male L. catta made an antipredator call, other group members vocalized in response. Dominant males did not make alarm calls at higher rates than subordinate males. Predators were more abundant on the western side of Parcel 1, but an even greater number of antipredator vocalizations occurred in this area than predator abundance warranted. We show that male L. catta consistently participated in group-level antipredator vocalization usage in high-risk locations. Although female L. catta are known to hold the primary role in group defense, male L. catta are also key participants in group-wide behaviors that may confuse or drive away predators.

1.
Alatalo R, Helle P (1990). Alarm calling by individual willow tits, Parus montanus. AnimalBehaviour 40: 437-442.
2.
Altmann J (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227-265.
3.
Baldellou M, Henzi S (1992). Vigilance, predator detection and the presence of supernumerary males in vervet monkey troops. Animal Behavior 43: 451-461.
4.
Bertram B (1978). Living in groups: predators and prey. In Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach (Krebs J, Davies N, eds.), pp 64-96. Oxford, Blackwell Scientific.
5.
Bolt L (2013a). The function of howling in the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). International Journal of Primatology 34: 157-169.
6.
Bolt L (2013b). The Relationship between Dominance and Vocal Communication in the Male Ring-Tailed Lemur (Lemur catta). PhD dissertation, University of Toronto.
7.
Bolt L (2013c). Squealing rate indicates dominance rank in the male ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). American Journal of Primatology 75: 1174-1184.
8.
Bolt L (2014). Male-specific use of the purr in the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Folia Primatologica 85: 201-214.
9.
Cheney D, Seyfarth R (1985). Vervet monkey alarm calls: manipulation through shared information? Behaviour 94: 150-166.
10.
Cresswell W (1994). The function of alarm calls in redshanks, Tringa tetanus. Animal Behaviour 47: 736-738.
11.
Cuozzo F, Sauther M (2006). Severe wear and tooth loss in wild ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta): a function of feeding ecology, dental structure, and individual life history. Journal of Human Evolution 51: 490-505.
12.
Cuozzo F, Sauther M, Gould L, Sussman R, Villers L, Lent C (2010). Variation in dental wear and tooth loss in known-aged, older ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta): a comparison between wild and captive individuals. American Journal of Primatology 72: 1026-1037.
13.
Ferrari M, Sih A, Chivers D (2009). The paradox of risk allocation: a review and prospectus. Animal Behavior 78: 579-585.
14.
Fichtel C (2007). Avoiding predators at night: antipredator strategies in red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus). American Journal of Primatology 69: 611-624.
15.
Giles N, Huntingford F (1984). Predation risk and inter-population variation in anti-predator behavior in the three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Animal Behaviour 32: 264-275.
16.
Gould L (1994). Patterns of Affiliative Behavior in Adult Male Ringtailed Lemurs (Lemur catta) at the Beza-Mahafaly Reserve, Madagascar. PhD dissertation, Washington University, St Louis.
17.
Gould L (1996). Vigilance behavior in naturally occurring ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta) during birth and lactation season. International Journal of Primatology 17: 331-347.
18.
Gould L, Sauther M (2006). Anti-predator strategies in a diurnal prosimian, the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta), at the Beza Mahafaly Special Reserve, Madagascar. In Primate Anti-Predator Strategies (Gursky S, Nekaris K, eds.), pp 275-288. New York, Springer.
19.
Gould L, Sussman R, Sauther M (2003). Demographic and life-history patterns in a population of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at Beza Mahafaly Reserve, Madagascar: a 15-year perspective. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 120: 182-194.
20.
Jolly A (1966). Lemur Behavior: A Madagascar Field Study. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
21.
Lewis R (2005). Sex differences in vigilance in Verreaux's sifaka: are males providing a predator detection service? American Journal of Physical Anthropology 40: 138.
22.
Lima S, Bednekoff P (1999). Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behavior: the predation risk allocation hypothesis. The American Naturalist 153: 649-659.
23.
Macedonia J (1990). What is communicated in the anti-predator calls of lemurs: evidence from playback experiments with ring-tailed and ruffed lemurs. Ethology 86: 177-190.
24.
Macedonia J (1993). The vocal repertoire of the ringtailed lemur (Lemur catta). Folia Primatologica 61: 186-217.
25.
Macedonia J, Evans C (1993). Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology 93: 177-197.
26.
Mirza R, Mathis A, Chivers D (2006). Does temporal variation in predation risk influence the intensity of antipredator responses? A test of the risk allocation hypothesis. Ethology 112: 44-51.
27.
Rose L, Fedigan L (1995). Vigilance in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus, in Costa Rica. Animal Behavior 49: 63-70.
28.
Sauther M (1989). Antipredator behavior in troops of free-ranging Lemur catta at Beza Mahafaly Special Reserve, Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology 10: 595-606.
29.
Sauther M, Cuozzo F (2008). Somatic variation in living, wild ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Folia Primatologica 79: 55-78.
30.
Sauther M, Sussman R, Cuozzo F (2002). Dental and general health in a population of wild ring-tailed lemurs: a life history approach. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 117: 122-132.
31.
Sherman P (1985). Alarm calls of Belding's ground squirrels to aerial predators: nepotism or self-preservation? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 17: 313-323.
32.
Sussman R, Ratsirarson J (2006). Beza Mahafaly Special Reserve: a research site in southwestern Madagascar. In Ringtailed Lemur Biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar (Jolly A, Sussman R, Koyama N, Rasamimanana H, eds.), pp 43-51. New York, Springer.
33.
Van Schaik C, van Noordwijk M (1989). The special role of male Cebus monkeys in predation avoidance and its effect on group composition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 24: 265-276.
34.
Wheeler B (2008). Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Animal Behaviour 76: 1465-1475.
35.
Woodward B (1983). Predator-prey interactions and breeding-pond use of temporary-pond species in a desert anuran community. Ecology 64: 1549-1555.
36.
Zuberbühler K, Jenny D, Bshary R (1999). The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 105: 477-490.
37.
Zumpe D, Michael R (1986). Dominance index: a simple measure of relative dominance status in primates. American Journal of Primatology 10: 291-300.
Copyright / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Copyright: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be translated into other languages, reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, microcopying, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.
You do not currently have access to this content.