Purpose: The purpose of this study was to establish and characterize age- and gender-specific normative data of the singing voice using the voice range profile for clinical diagnostics. Furthermore, associations between the singing voice and the socioeconomic status were examined. Methods: Singing voice profiles of 1,578 mostly untrained children aged between 7.0 and 16.11 years were analyzed. Participants had to reproduce sung tones at defined pitches, resulting in maximum and minimum fundamental frequency and sound pressure level (SPL). In addition, maximum phonation time (MPT) was measured. Percentile curves of frequency, SPL and MPT were estimated. To examine the associations of socioeconomic status, multivariate analyses adjusted for age and sex were performed. Results: In boys, the mean of the highest frequency was 750.9 Hz and lowered to 397.1 Hz with increasing age. Similarly, the minimum frequency was 194.4 Hz and lowered to 91.9 Hz. In girls, the mean maximum frequency decreased from 754.9 to 725.3 Hz. The mean minimum frequency lowered from 202.4 to 175.0 Hz. For both sexes, the mean frequency range ∆f showed a constant range of roughly 24 semitones. The MPT increased with age, for boys and girls. There was neither an effect of age nor sex on SPLmin or SPLmax, ranging between 52.6 and 54.1 dBA and between 86.5 and 82.8 dBA, respectively. Socioeconomic status was not associated with the above-mentioned variables. Conclusion: To our knowledge, this study is the first to present large normative data on the singing voice in childhood and adolescence based on a high number of measurements. In addition, we provide percentile curves for practical application in clinic and vocal pedagogy which may be applied to distinguish between normal and pathological singing voice.

The voice is an integral part of communication, enabling social interaction in adults and children alike. During childhood and adolescence, processes like growth, maturation, and puberty have an important influence on the development of a child’s voice. Vocal disorders during that time can have a severe impact on a child’s social behavior and academic achievement, possibly leading to higher unemployment rates in later life [1-3]. Those ailments are not rare as prevalence in children is estimated at 6% [4], other studies even stating a percentage as high as 24%, especially in urban areas [5, 6]. These vocal illnesses may present themselves as throat clearing or coughing, the voice becomes strained or tires, the voice gets low or hoarse, difficulty in being heard, sensation of tension, pain, or lump in the throat, and voice breaks while talking. The most common manifestation in childhood is a hyperfunctional voice disorder with vocal nodules as secondary organic changes of the epithelium [6, 7]. Therefore, detecting these illnesses should be a focus of phoniatricians and pediatricians.

Previously, study populations were mostly too small to derive normative data, some consider exclusively vocally trained or untrained children or specific age groups like infants or boys during voice change [2, 8, 9]. Due to the lack of normative data, the interpretation of the singing voice remains subjective. This study aimed to establish normative data on the singing voice of mostly untrained children and adolescents, who represent a typical distribution of singing activity in a normal population. Due to an exceptionally high number of 1,578 of participants, solid normative data could be created.

The voice range profile (VRP) is a common diagnostic tool to qualify and quantify basic parameters of the singing and speaking voice. Titze [10] described it as a standard nonspeech task for the assessment of laryngeal motor control to assess the pitch and the loudness. Being a noninvasive, semiobjective tool, it is useful to visualize the extent of the frequencies someone may produce sung at low intensity and high intensity, thus showing the dimension of the vocal frequency and intensity. It might assist the diagnosis of voice problems. Accordingly, irregular findings might indicate voice disorders like hoarseness, glottal chinks, and nodules or functional misuse of the voice [11, 12].

As this study aims to provide normative data, percentile curves have been estimated as well. Percentile curves are commonly used in pediatrics to visualize the age-varying distribution of a parameter. Therefore, the method facilitates the assessment of an individual’s performance in the context of the sex- and age-adjusted norm.

In addition to frequency and intensity ranges this study establishes normative data on the maximum phonation time (MPT) which is used as an indicator of phonatory control [13]. Furthermore, a reduction of MPT is a measure of voice impairments according to the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health. Hillel et al. [14] suggest that any shortening of the MPT indicates potential vocal disabilities, like vocal nodules, chronic laryngitis vocal polyps, and others [15]; therefore, it can be used to assess an individual’s voice health status [16].

Besides the general description of the voice profile, the relationship between socioeconomic status and children’s voice was assessed. So far, only a few studies have investigated possible associations even though social background plays an important role in health. In lower-income families deficient communication culture due to the lack of a framework for objective argumentation, or patient listening, as well as high noise level in the family environment, is more common [17]. This might lead to long-term overstraining of vocal capabilities and thus to the development of vocal disorders [18]. These traits might also be found in larger families, disregarding their social status. Moreover, associations between a child’s speaking-voice intensity and the personality, especially extraversion and emotional stability, which are more common in families with higher socioeconomic background, were described by Poulain et al. [19]. Smillie et al. [20] state that socioeconomic status has an impact on the children’s speaking voice, with children from lower social backgrounds showing more vocal disorders. Therefore, this study tries to show whether socioeconomic status has an influence on the singing voice.

Study Population

This study is part of the LIFE Child Study, an ongoing large prospective cohort study, which started in 2011 (clinical trial No. NCT02550236) [21, 22]. The LIFE Child Study has been designed to understand how (epi)genetic, metabolic, and environmental factors influence health and development in children and adolescents in modern society. More than 4,000 children, adolescents, and their families have been recruited so far. The VRP examinations were conducted to gain information on the natural development and variation of voice parameters during childhood and adolescence. The term adolescence used in this study describes participants at the beginning of puberty up to the age of majority of 18 years, as in most countries. Between 2011 and 2016 one VRP measurement of each of the 1,578 children aged between 7.0 and 16.11 years was collected. 91 measurements had to be excluded as described below. Thus, the analyses are based on a total of 1,487 measurements.

Voice Measurement

The VRP included speaking voice as well as singing voice tasks. The speaking voice of the participants was analyzed by Berger et al. [23] and will not be a part of this study. The singing voice was assessed with frequency and intensity parameters while reproducing defined pitches presented by the examiner using an electric piano, thus we describe a specific function or phonatory capabilities of the larynx. The sequence of tones started at a pitch in the middle of the speaking voice range of each subject lowering down to a minimum and advancing subsequently to higher frequencies until a maximum was reached. First, the subject was asked to sing as softly as possible, afterward as loudly as possible. If a participant was able to use the high register, this was included in the measurement. Throughout the recording, the investigators provided the subjects with hand signals to coach them for further lowering/increasing their volume. The examiner was allowed to give instructions and advice even during the recording, as deemed necessary. In case of software malfunction or vocal failure, the examiner could delete the respective recording and start a second attempt. The VRP visualizes each frequency and their correspondent intensities.

In addition, the subject was instructed to hold a sung tone for as long as possible within a comfortable pitch and loudness quantifying the MPT. To exclude any training effect of the MPT, this procedure was only done once.

All examiners had been trained and certified by phoniatricians. Furthermore, to ensure high quality over the entire study period and to increase examiner reliability, they were periodically (every 3 months) full-time recertified and supervised by the above-mentioned specialists. To reduce external noise all measurements were performed in a soundproof room. Ambient noise did not exceed 40 dB according to the standard operating procedures recommended by the Union of the European Phoniatricians [24]. Singing voices were recorded using DiVAS® software (XION Medical, Berlin, Germany) running on a portable Windows-based PC. It includes a self-calibrating XION USB-microphone-headset with a constant distance of 30 cm from the subject’s mouth, which secures the exact measurement of the parameters, without prior calibration. Unlike adults, who are supposed to stand at a defined spot, the participant could rest on his parent’s lap in order to reduce timidity or movements during the examination. Those comprehensive standard operating procedures ensure a high and stable quality of the registration of the voice parameters.

In this study, the highest and lowest fundamental frequency (f0max [Hz], f0min [Hz]) were taken into account. The difference between both, ∆f [semitones] (f0max – f0min), that is, the frequency range of an individual, was expressed in semitones using the scientific pitch notation (A0 = a’ = 440 Hz) to allow a straightforward comparison. Furthermore, the sound pressure level (SPL [dBA]), was expressed by the loudest (SPLmax) and the softest (SPLmin) tone sung by each participant, and their margin ∆SPL was described. Finally, the MPT (s) was assessed.

Exclusion Criteria

To ensure a high data quality 91 measurements had to be excluded. To obtain reliable and analyzable data on a singing voice >4 tones sung softly and another 4 sung loudly needed to be accurately recorded for each child. All measurements were assessed individually, not only visually, but also audibly. In the course of this the accuracy of the pitch-matching was assessed. 72 measurements did not meet the prerequisites. In addition, 12 children were not vocally healthy at the day of examination, having an upper respiratory infection or were hoarse. 7 measurements had to be removed due to technical problems. Hence of the total of 1,578 measurements, 1,487 could be evaluated.

Sociodemographics and Singing Activity

To determine the socioeconomic status of each participant the adjusted Winkler index was applied, taking into account education and occupational status of the parents as well as the equivalent household income. The resulting score can be classified into upper, middle, and lower class [25, 26]. The children, if needed with the help of their parents, were asked via a standardized questionnaire following the classification by Fuchs et al. [27] about their degree of vocal strain and vocal training and whether they played a wind instrument, as there are indications in the literature that this may lead to increased strain on the vocal apparatus. Based on the classification of singing activity defined by Fuchs et al. [27], the study population showed that 90.8% of the participating children had no voice training, and sing only spontaneously and not in front of an audience. 9.2% of the children engage in occasional or regular organized singing and have voice training in a large group up to individual lessons. All children were included while estimating reference percentiles because this composition is typical for a western population.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using R for Macintosh version 3.4.1 (RStudio Inc., Boston, MA, USA). Linear regression and multiple linear regression models were used to analyze the association of age, sex, and socioeconomic status as independent variables. p values ≤0.05 were considered to be statistically significant. To estimate percentile curves of the voice frequency an LMS-type method implemented as a generalized additive model for location, scale, and shape was applied (GAMLSS). The method is recommended by the World Health Organization for generating age-dependent reference values. It was used in the World Health Organization multicenter growth study to establish the new international growth standard [28, 29].

Ethics

The study was designed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and under the supervision of the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig (Reg. No. 264-10-19042010). Each participant and the authorized representative were informed about the study program, the long-term use of data, potential risks of participation, and the right to withdraw from the study. Informed written consent was provided by all parents and, from the age of 12 years, by the children themselves.

Data Summary

In summary 1,487 measurements were considered. Of those 756 (50.8%) were male and 731 (49.2%) female. Table 1 shows the voice characteristics of the study population stratified by age.

Table 1.

Voice characteristics of the study population stratified by age

Voice characteristics of the study population stratified by age
Voice characteristics of the study population stratified by age

Frequency: Effects of Age and Sex

The mean highest fundamental frequency for males as well as the mean lowest fundamental frequency of male singing voices did not change significantly up to the age of 12 (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.32). During voice change, it lowered significantly until the mean lowest and highest frequencies reach a stable level at the age of 15. The largest slope might be seen for males during the voice change between 12 and 15 years of age where the maximum frequency decreased by 117.6 Hz (95% CI 145.0–90.2; p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.45); at 12 it was 684.9 Hz and at 15 years 402.1 Hz. In males the mean highest frequency lowered from 750.9 Hz (corresponding roughly to G5) at the age of 7 to 397.1 Hz (G4) at the age of 16 years (p ≤ 0.05, d = 0.74). Similarly the minimum frequency lowered from 194.4 Hz (G3) at the age of 7 to 91.9 Hz (GЬ2) at the age of 16 (p ≤ 0.05,d =0.63). Thus, male voices lower by around one octave during voice change.

The mean highest fundamental frequency of female singing voices changed significantly with increasing age. It shows, in contrast to male singing voices, a linear lowering of 8.2 Hz (95% CI 11.9–4.6) per year (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.152). Likewise, the mean lowest fundamental frequency of female singing voices lowered 3.0 Hz (95% CI 3.6–2.4) per year (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.143). In females the mean maximum frequency also lowers continuously and significantly, but not starkly, from 754.9 (G5) at the age of 7 to 725.3 (F♯5) at the age of 16 years (p ≤ 0.05, d = 0.53). A similar trend can be observed for the minimum frequency from 202.4 Hz (AЬ3) at the age of 7 to 175.0 Hz (F3) at the age of 16 years. Female voices lower approximately 2 semitones per year during adolescence.

Figure 1 shows the percentile curves for f0max and f0min in males and females. Note the decrease of the margin for frequency at the age of 12 years for males.

Fig. 1.

Percentile curves for the variables f0max for males (a), females (b), and f0min for males (c) and females (d). The y axes depict on the left-hand side the frequencies and on the right-hand side the corresponding scientific pitch notations.

Fig. 1.

Percentile curves for the variables f0max for males (a), females (b), and f0min for males (c) and females (d). The y axes depict on the left-hand side the frequencies and on the right-hand side the corresponding scientific pitch notations.

Close modal

Figure 2 shows the percentile curves for the variable ∆f which is the difference between f0max and f0min. ∆f is expressed in semitones to ensure an easy comparison. There was no significant age trend in males and females. The frequency range of around 24 semitones (2 octaves) stays stable over age.

Fig. 2.

The percentile curves for ∆f for males (a) and females (b) depict the development of the variable over age.

Fig. 2.

The percentile curves for ∆f for males (a) and females (b) depict the development of the variable over age.

Close modal

As expected, the frequency lowers with age which can be attributed to adolescence for both sexes. Sex has a steady effect on the frequencies. There is a significant difference independent from age with males showing a lower maximum and minimum frequency at all ages (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.57; Table 1).

SPL: Effects of Age and Sex

In males the mean SPLmax of the singing voice lowers significantly over age. It shows a linear lowering of 0.05 dBA (95% CI 0.11–0.05) per year (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.124). A similar development can be seen for SPLmin with a decrease of 0.15 dBA (95% CI 0.23–0.06) per year (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.117).

In females the means of SPLmax (b = –0.001 [95% CI –0.12 to 0.12], p = 0.63) and SPLmin (b = –0.34 (95% CI –0.12 to 0.05), p = 0.44) show no significant age trends.

Figure 3 shows the percentile curves of the SPLmax and SPLmin for both sexes.

Fig. 3.

Percentile curves for the variables SPLmax for males (a), females (b), and SPLmin for males (c) and females (d) depict the development of the variable over age.

Fig. 3.

Percentile curves for the variables SPLmax for males (a), females (b), and SPLmin for males (c) and females (d) depict the development of the variable over age.

Close modal

Maximum Phonation Time

The variable MPT significantly increased over age. In males it increased by 0.6 s (95% CI 0.5–0.7) per year (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.42), in females it increased in a less pronounced manner by around 0.4 s (95% CI 0.3–0.4) per year (p ≤ 0.05, f2 = 0.36). We found a significant difference between the sexes with males showing significantly longer MPT than females (p ≤ 0.05, d = 0.43; Fig. 4a). In 7-year-old children, boys were able to hold a sung tone for an average of 11.1 s; being at the age of 16 years, they were able to hold it for around 15.9 s (p ≤ 0.05, d = 0.76). Similar effects can be seen in females showing an average of 10.7 s at the age of 7 compared to 14.4 s at the age of 16 years (p ≤ 0.05, d = 0.82). Figure 4 shows the respective percentile curves.

Fig. 4.

The percentile curves for MPT for males (a) and females (b) depict the development of the variable over age.

Fig. 4.

The percentile curves for MPT for males (a) and females (b) depict the development of the variable over age.

Close modal

Effects of Winkler Score

The score was only available for 811 participants due to missing data. The age and sex distributions of the subsample did not differ from those of the entire sample. 27.1% of the participants belong to the lower-income families, 66.8% to the middle and 6.1% to the upper class, which resembles the distribution in Germany. For f0min, f0max, and ∆f there were no significant differences between the socioeconomic classes. In general, there were also no significant differences between the socioeconomic classes for SPLmin and SPLmax. Socioeconomic status shows no influence on the variable MPT.

The aim of this present study was to amass normative data on the singing voice of children and adolescents. Having an unmatched large group of examinations of children aged between 7 and 16.11 gives this unique opportunity to create solid normative data.

Siupsinskiene and Lycke [30] describe that it is important for otorhinolaryngologists or phoniatricians to have a quantification of voice quality in order to be able to assess any vocal pathology. This study provides a high quality of measurements and consecutively equally valuable normative data: first, by implementing standard operating procedures as described above; second, by the regular supervising of the measurement process by trained phoniatricians, and third, by re-evaluating the given data in a lengthy process both visibly and audibly. This study helps to quantify the voice quality and describe a healthy voice. The percentile curves of these variables might help visualize the development and give a tool for appraising the singing voice. A comparison with previous findings might be complex, as most previous studies have used the fundamental frequency to depict the frequency of each individual by letting a subject read set texts [31-33]. Here, one goes further by describing the minimum frequency as well as the maximum frequency. It gives a much broader view on the children’s singing voice. The authors also believe that a frequency range as done here shows the extent of a healthy voice of a general population of children and adolescents, following the findings of Wuyts et al. [5]. Alterations especially in the sense of a limited vocal range may be an indication of possible vocal disorders and lead to further clinical examination.

This study shows values for the minimum frequency for boys of 194.4 Hz (G3) at the age of 7 and similar ones for females of the same age (202.4 Hz [AЬ3]). These findings resemble the data of nonsinging children described by Siupsinskiene and Lycke [30]. The contrast of frequencies between the sexes is easily visible when comparing 16-year-old males with a minimum frequency of 81.9 Hz (GЬ2) and females with 175.0 Hz (F3), the latter resembling the values found by Lycke and Siupsinskiene [34] for female nonsingers. A similar descent is shown with the variable maximum frequency as depicted above. The expectable effect of voice change is seen here for both males and females. The lowering of the male voice goes hand in hand with other data of the boy’s voice during voice change [35-37]. It can be described by roughly one octave. This is true for the female voice as well, but not as strongly, as it only lowers by 2 semitones [38, 39]. The frequency range of about 24 semitones stays constant during ageing and is the same for females and males which is shown by other authors as well [11, 12, 30]. As >90% of the participants are nonsingers the authors of this study deem these frequency variables as appropriate to describe voices of vocally healthy children in a population of mostly vocally inactive people.

The SPL as a parameter to describe the voice of an individual has been used by other authors as well. Their findings resemble the ones depicted in this study [8]. The minimum SPL in this study is found to be around 53 dBA and the maximum SPL 85 dBA. Schneider-Stickler [40] described that a healthy voice should reach 90 dBA, even if other studies found lower values [8]. Others describe slightly higher values, even up to 97 dBA [11, 41]. Reasons for this gap in SPLmax could be due to the motivation of the children and the study situation where the collection of voice parameters was only one part of the many aspects and examinations of the LIFE Child Study.

MPT as an important variable to describe the healthy voice is an indicator of phonatory control [14]. In this study normative data for boys aged 7 years shows values for MPT at 11.1 s, for females 10.7 s which supports findings of other authors [42]. When growing up, MPT extends for boys up to 15.9 s and for girls to 14.4 s. This can also be seen in values described by Fuchs et al. [43], who stated that the MPT for boys is 3 s longer than for females. This can be explained by the physical development during puberty, as a child’s growth generally increases lung capacity [44]. Some authors reckon that various morbidities tend to shorten the MPT [14, 16, 45], others discuss the influence of singing activity on the MPT. Yet, Wendler et al. [46] state that too much focus is put on the measurement of the MPT. The difference of male and female voices might also be contributed to the fact that females have a higher prevalence of posterior chinks and these are quite common during puberty [47]. The authors of this study, though, reckon that it is necessary to have reliable data on MPT. Even if not decisive to discriminate between vocally healthy and invalid children, they at least provide an auxiliary mean to support any suspicion. Further studies are needed to validate the implementation of MPT as an appraisal for a healthy voice. The percentile curves give a tool to appraise the MPT of a subject quickly and easily.

Similarly to pediatrics, these percentile curves, not only of the variable MPT, give an overview about individual results and may be used to compare them with those of other children. It thereby adds great value to the clinical routine of a phoniatrician and pediatrician as with the help of this acquired normative data one is able to find irregularities with a fast and noninvasive examination.

Socioeconomic class as an important influence for a child’s health has been widely described [48, 49]. This led to the inclusion of socioeconomic data in this study to determine whether any differences between the social class regarding the children’s voice may be described. Here socioeconomic status is depicted in 3 classes as described above. Other studies describe an influence regarding vocal fold nodules [20] or communication disorders in general [3]. Those described data in another way, so any comparison might be complex. Also, there are significant confounders when associating singing voice with socioeconomic status, like the number of people living in a family or the access to health care, which were not addressed in this study [17, 50]. The data given in this study established no significant differences between the 3 social classes regarding all the parameters. One might conclude that although social class influences other ENT- and phoniatrics-related diseases, it does not have an impact on the extent and characteristics of a child’s singing voice.

In this study the authors only took data from an area in Germany. To describe singing voice on a global scale multicentric studies need to be conducted. In addition to that, the authors did not check for a range of ethnicities or native languages so the normative data might be only suitable for Caucasian subjects due to limited representativeness [51, 52]. Further studies need to be conducted to show whether the VRP is the preferable tool for assessing vocal disorders. Musical deficits or prodigy might interfere with the diagnosis. Also, this study might be limited due to the fact that no further distinction was drawn between singers and nonsingers. The participants represent the typical distribution of singing activity in a normal population. Of course, individual conditions, like regular singing in choirs, can lead to a significant extension of the parameters. Additionally, the authors think to have a profound insight of the voice of a child one needs to assess both speaking and singing voice. Further correlations between those 2 are in need to be examined. Important information might be added in a longitudinal study as this study is a cross-sectional one.

This present study describes reliable data of the singing voice of children with the variables f0min, f0max, ∆f, SPLmin, SPLmax, ∆SPL, and MPT for the first time. Its validity is substantiated by its unmatched high number of measurements. Using the VRP as a tool to examine a child’s voice at all ages seems feasible and reliable for gathering the vocal dimension of each subject. Thus, these data allow a valid appraisal of reference values for the given variables of the singing voice of mostly vocally inactive people. The different socioeconomic backgrounds present in society seem to have no effect on vocal parameters in children without voice disorders. With this gained normative data the professional who deals with children’s voices is able to distinguish between normal parameters of the singing voice and parameters that need further investigations to follow and may intervene early if necessary.

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants are in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards and under the supervision of the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig (Reg. No. 264-10-19042010). Informed written consent was provided by all parents and, from the age of 12 years, by the children themselves.

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

This publication is supported by LIFE–Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases, University of Leipzig. LIFE is funded by means of the European Union, by means of the European Social Fund (ESF), by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF), and by means of the Free State of Saxony within the framework of the excellence initiative. The Integrated Research and Treatment Center Adiposity Diseases is funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (grant 01EO1501). XION Medical (Berlin, Germany) kindly provided the technical equipment and software for conducting voice measurements.

T.B., T.D., M.F., and W.K. conceived of the presented idea. T.B., M.F., and W.K. conceived and planned the experiments. The LIFE Child Research Center carried out the experiments. T.D. performed the computations. C.E., T. Peschel, T. Poulain, and M.V. verified the analytical methods. T.D. took the lead in writing the paper. All authors provided critical feedback and helped shape the research, analysis, and paper.

1.
Connor
NP
,
Cohen
SB
,
Theis
SM
,
Thibeault
SL
,
Heatley
DG
,
Bless
DM
.
Attitudes of children with dysphonia
.
J Voice
.
2008
Mar
;
22
(
2
):
197
209
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
2.
Infusino
SA
,
Diercks
GR
,
Rogers
DJ
,
Garcia
J
,
Ojha
S
,
Maurer
R
, et al
Establishment of a normative cepstral pediatric acoustic database
.
JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg
.
2015
Apr
;
141
(
4
):
358
63
.
[PubMed]
2168-6181
3.
R. J.
Ruben
, ‘
Valedictory—why pediatric otorhinolaryngology is important
’, Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol., vol. 67, no. Supplement 1, pp. S53–S61,
Dec.
2003
, doi: .
4.
Carding
PN
,
Roulstone
S
,
Northstone
K
;
ALSPAC Study Team
.
The prevalence of childhood dysphonia: a cross-sectional study
.
J Voice
.
2006
Dec
;
20
(
4
):
623
30
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
5.
Wuyts
FL
,
Heylen
L
,
Mertens
F
,
Du Caju
M
,
Rooman
R
,
Van de Heyning
PH
, et al
Effects of age, sex, and disorder on voice range profile characteristics of 230 children
.
Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol
.
2003
Jun
;
112
(
6
):
540
8
.
[PubMed]
0003-4894
6.
Kallvik
E
,
Lindström
E
,
Holmqvist
S
,
Lindman
J
,
Simberg
S
.
Prevalence of hoarseness in school-aged children
.
J Voice
.
2015
Mar
;
29
(
2
):
260.e1
19
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
7.
Simberg
S
,
Sala
E
,
Laine
A
,
Rönnemaa
AM
.
A fast and easy screening method for voice disorders among teacher students
.
Logoped Phoniatr Vocol
.
2001
;
26
(
1
):
10
6
.
[PubMed]
1401-5439
8.
Böhme
G
,
Stuchlik
G
.
Voice profiles and standard voice profile of untrained children
.
J Voice
.
1995
Sep
;
9
(
3
):
304
7
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
9.
Pedersen
MF
.
A longitudinal pilot study on phonetograms/voice profiles in pre-pubertal choir boys
.
Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci
.
1993
Dec
;
18
(
6
):
488
91
.
[PubMed]
0307-7772
10.
Titze
IR
.
The G. Paul Moore Lecture. Toward standards in acoustic analysis of voice
.
J Voice
.
1994
Mar
;
8
(
1
):
1
7
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
11.
Schneider
B
,
Zumtobel
M
,
Prettenhofer
W
,
Aichstill
B
,
Jocher
W
.
Normative voice range profiles in vocally trained and untrained children aged between 7 and 10 years
.
J Voice
.
2010
Mar
;
24
(
2
):
153
60
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
12.
McAllister
A
,
Sederholm
E
,
Sundberg
J
,
Gramming
P
.
Relations between voice range profiles and physiological and perceptual voice characteristics in ten-year-old children
.
J Voice
.
1994
Sep
;
8
(
3
):
230
9
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
13.
Hirano
M
,
McCormick
KR
.
Clinical Examination of Voice by Minoru Hirano
.
J Acoust Soc Am
.
1986
Oct
;
80
(
4
):
1273
1273
. 0001-4966
14.
Hillel
AD
,
Yorkston
K
,
Miller
RM
.
Using phonation time to estimate vital capacity in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis
.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
.
1989
Aug
;
70
(
8
):
618
20
.
[PubMed]
0003-9993
15.
Ma
EP
,
Yiu
EM
.
Multiparametric evaluation of dysphonic severity
.
J Voice
.
2006
Sep
;
20
(
3
):
380
90
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
16.
Prathanee
B
,
Sae Soew
P
,
Pongjanyakul
A
,
Sae-Heng
S
;
PRATHANEE
.
P. S. SOEW, A. PONGJANYAKUL, and S. SAE-HENG, ‘Time and frequency of maximum phonation of normal Thai children in Khon Kaen’
.
J Multilingual Commun Disord
.
2003
Jan
;
1
(
1
):
71
8
. 1476-9670
17.
Bradley
RH
,
Corwyn
RF
.
Socioeconomic status and child development
.
Annu Rev Psychol
.
2002
;
53
(
1
):
371
99
.
[PubMed]
0066-4308
18.
Qi
CH
,
Kaiser
AP
.
Problem behaviors of low-income children with language delays: an observation study
.
J Speech Lang Hear Res
.
2004
Jun
;
47
(
3
):
595
609
.
[PubMed]
1092-4388
19.
Poulain
T
, et al
Associations of Speaking-Voice Parameters With Personality and Behavior in School-Aged Children
.
J Voice
.
2018
;(
Oct
):
[PubMed]
0892-1997
20.
Smillie
I
,
McManus
K
,
Cohen
W
,
Lawson
E
,
Wynne
DM
.
The paediatric voice clinic
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2014
Oct
;
99
(
10
):
912
5
.
[PubMed]
0003-9888
21.
Poulain
T
,
Baber
R
,
Vogel
M
,
Pietzner
D
,
Kirsten
T
,
Jurkutat
A
, et al;
LIFE Child study team
.
The LIFE Child study: a population-based perinatal and pediatric cohort in Germany
.
Eur J Epidemiol
.
2017
Feb
;
32
(
2
):
145
58
.
[PubMed]
0393-2990
22.
Quante
M
,
Hesse
M
,
Döhnert
M
,
Fuchs
M
,
Hirsch
C
,
Sergeyev
E
, et al;
LIFE Child Study Investigators
.
The LIFE child study: a life course approach to disease and health
.
BMC Public Health
.
2012
Nov
;
12
(
1
):
1021
.
[PubMed]
1471-2458
23.
Berger
T
, et al
Speaking Voice in Children and Adolescents: Normative Data and Associations with BMI, Tanner Stage, and Singing Activity
.
J Voice
.
2018
May
;
0
(
0
):
[PubMed]
0892-1997
24.
Schutte
HK
,
Seidner
W
.
Recommendation by the Union of European Phoniatricians (UEP): standardizing voice area measurement/phonetography
.
Folia Phoniatr (Basel)
.
1983
;
35
(
6
):
286
8
.
[PubMed]
0015-5705
25.
Lampert
T
,
Müters
S
,
Stolzenberg
H
,
Kroll
LE
. ‘
Messung des sozioökonomischen Status in der KiGGS-Studie
’,
Jun.
2014
, Accessed: Dec. 10, 2017. [Online]. Available: http://edoc.rki.de/docviews/abstract.php?lang=ger&id=3569
26.
Winkler
J
,
Stolzenberg
H
.
Adjustierung des Sozialen-Schicht-Index für die Anwendung im Kinder-und Jugendgesundheitssurvey (KiGGS)
.
Wismarer Diskussionspapiere
;
2009
.
27.
Fuchs
M
,
Meuret
S
,
Geister
D
,
Pfohl
W
,
Thiel
S
,
Dietz
A
, et al
Empirical criteria for establishing a classification of singing activity in children and adolescents
.
J Voice
.
2008
Nov
;
22
(
6
):
649
57
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
28.
Rigby
RA
,
Stasinopoulos
DM
.
Generalized additive models for location, scale and shape
.
J R Stat Soc Ser C Appl Stat
.
2005
Jun
;
54
(
3
):
507
54
. 1467-9876
29.
Smoothing reference centile curves: The lms method and penalized likelihood - Cole - 1992 - Statistics in Medicine - Wiley Online Library
’. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/sim.4780111005 (accessed Aug. 13, 2019).
30.
Siupsinskiene
N
,
Lycke
H
.
Effects of vocal training on singing and speaking voice characteristics in vocally healthy adults and children based on choral and nonchoral data
.
J Voice
.
2011
Jul
;
25
(
4
):
e177
89
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
31.
Guzman
M
,
Muñoz
D
,
Vivero
M
,
Marín
N
,
Ramírez
M
,
Rivera
MT
, et al
Acoustic markers to differentiate gender in prepubescent children’s speaking and singing voice
.
Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol
.
2014
Oct
;
78
(
10
):
1592
8
.
[PubMed]
0165-5876
32.
Sorenson
DN
.
A fundamental frequency investigation of children ages 6-10 years old
.
J Commun Disord
.
1989
Apr
;
22
(
2
):
115
23
.
[PubMed]
0021-9924
33.
Lee
S
,
Potamianos
A
,
Narayanan
S
.
Acoustics of children’s speech: developmental changes of temporal and spectral parameters
.
J Acoust Soc Am
.
1999
Mar
;
105
(
3
):
1455
68
.
[PubMed]
0001-4966
34.
Lycke
H
,
Siupsinskiene
N
.
Voice Range Profiles of Singing Students: The Effects of Training Duration and Institution
.
Folia Phoniatr Logop
.
2016
;
68
(
2
):
53
9
.
[PubMed]
1021-7762
35.
Hollien
H
.
On pubescent voice change in males
.
J Voice
.
2012
Mar
;
26
(
2
):
e29
40
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
36.
Hollien
H
,
Green
R
,
Massey
K
.
Longitudinal research on adolescent voice change in males
.
J Acoust Soc Am
.
1994
Nov
;
96
(
5 Pt 1
):
2646
54
.
[PubMed]
0001-4966
37.
Naidr
J
,
Zbořil
M
,
Ševčík
K
.
Die pubertalen Veränderungen der Stimme bei Jungen im Verlauf von 5 Jahren
.
Folia Phoniatr Logop
.
1965
;
17
(
1
):
1
18
. 1021-7762
38.
Harries
ML
,
Walker
JM
,
Williams
DM
,
Hawkins
S
,
Hughes
IA
.
Changes in the male voice at puberty
.
Arch Dis Child
.
1997
Nov
;
77
(
5
):
445
7
.
[PubMed]
0003-9888
39.
Spiegel
JR
,
Sataloff
RT
,
Emerich
KA
.
The young adult voice
.
J Voice
.
1997
Jun
;
11
(
2
):
138
43
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
40.
Schneider-Stickler
B
.
Stimmstörungen im Kindesalter
.
HNO
.
2012
Jul
;
60
(
7
):
590
4
.
[PubMed]
0017-6192
41.
Brockmann-Bauser
M
,
Beyer
D
,
Bohlender
JE
.
Reliable acoustic measurements in children between 5;0 and 9;11 years: Gender, age, height and weight effects on fundamental frequency, jitter and shimmer in phonations without and with controlled voice SPL
.
Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol
.
2015
Dec
;
79
(
12
):
2035
42
.
[PubMed]
0165-5876
42.
Fuchs
M
,
Heide
S
,
Hentschel
B
,
Gelbrich
G
,
Makuch
A
,
Thiel
S
, et al
Stimmleistungsparameter bei Kindern und JugendlichenVocal efficiency parameters in children and adolescents
.
HNO
.
2006
Dec
;
54
(
12
):
971
80
.
[PubMed]
0017-6192
43.
Fuchs
M
,
Meuret
S
,
Thiel
S
,
Täschner
R
,
Dietz
A
,
Gelbrich
G
.
Influence of singing activity, age, and sex on voice performance parameters, on subjects’ perception and use of their voice in childhood and adolescence
.
J Voice
.
2009
Mar
;
23
(
2
):
182
9
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
44.
Finnegan
DE
.
Maximum phonation time for children with normal voices
.
Folia Phoniatr (Basel)
.
1985
;
37
(
5-6
):
209
15
.
[PubMed]
0015-5705
45.
Harden
JR
,
Looney
NA
.
Duration of sustained phonation in kindergarten children
.
Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol
.
1984
Mar
;
7
(
1
):
11
9
.
[PubMed]
0165-5876
46.
J.
Wendler
,
W.
Seidner
,
U.
Eysholdt
, ‘Lehrbuch der Phoniatrie und Padaudiologie, 3rd ed.’.
Logoped Phoniatr Vocol
.
1998
Jan
;
23
(
1
):
44
6
. 1401-5439
47.
Chandran
S
,
Hanna
J
,
Lurie
D
,
Sataloff
RT
.
Differences between flexible and rigid endoscopy in assessing the posterior glottic chink
.
J Voice
.
2011
Sep
;
25
(
5
):
591
5
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
48.
Holstein
BE
,
Jørgensen
SE
,
Due
P
,
Damsgaard
MT
,
Rasmussen
M
.
Short report: persistent social inequality in poor self-rated health among adolescents in Denmark 1991-2014
.
Eur J Public Health
.
2018
Dec
;
28
(
6
):
1114
6
.
[PubMed]
1101-1262
49.
Adler
NE
,
Glymour
MM
,
Fielding
J
.
Addressing Social Determinants of Health and Health Inequalities
.
JAMA
.
2016
Oct
;
316
(
16
):
1641
2
.
[PubMed]
0098-7484
50.
Espenshade
TJ
,
Kamenske
G
,
Turchi
BA
.
Family size and economic welfare
.
Fam Plann Perspect
.
1983
Nov-Dec
;
15
(
6
):
289
94
.
[PubMed]
0014-7354
51.
Andrianopoulos
AV
,
Darrow
KN
,
Chen
J
.
Multimodal standardization of voice among four multicultural populations: fundamental frequency and spectral characteristics
.
J Voice
.
2001
Jun
;
15
(
2
):
194
219
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
52.
Xue
SA
,
Hao
JG
.
Normative standards for vocal tract dimensions by race as measured by acoustic pharyngometry
.
J Voice
.
2006
Sep
;
20
(
3
):
391
400
.
[PubMed]
0892-1997
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.