Background: Fasting changes mood and physiological states. Substance use, such as khat use, is prohibited during fasting during Ramadan, a traditional practice among Muslims. Habitual khat use is associated with increased negative affect and altered psychobiological stress responses. Effects of fasting on stress responses, mood, and withdrawal symptoms among khat users have not been examined. Methods: In this study, 80 individuals completed an ambulatory monitoring period and a laboratory assessment session. Participants who completed the study while fasting during Ramadan were matched by gender and khat use status with participants who completed the study while not fasting. This resulted in 40 participants (12 females and 28 males; 25 khat users and 15 nonusers) in each fasting group. Cardiovascular (blood pressure and heart rate) and subjective measures were collected throughout the laboratory stress session. A mental arithmetic challenge was used to induce stress. In addition, self-reported mood and withdrawal measures were collected multiple times during the ambulatory assessment. Results: Khat users reported greater negative affect than nonusers. Results from the ambulatory study indicated that withdrawal symptoms were lower during evening hours in the fasting group than in the no-fasting group. Stress-related changes in positive and negative affects were flattened in the fasting group relative to the no-fasting group. Khat users reported reduced blood pressure responses relative to nonusers. Conclusion: These preliminary results demonstrate that fasting is associated with reduced negative affect and withdrawal symptoms in khat users. Khat use was related to blunted blood pressure stress responses, but this was independent of fasting. Due to the small sample size, these results should be replicated with a large sample and comprehensive stress tasks.

Khat is a natural stimulant plant that is available in parts of East Africa and the southern part of the Arabian Peninsula [1]. Cathinone, a phenylethylamine alkaloid, is a major constituent of khat that mediates khat’s psychostimulant properties by mainly activating dopaminergic and noradrenergic pathways, though it also activates the serotonergic pathway to a lesser extent [2-5]. Cathinone chemically resembles amphetamine [2]. Historically, khat sessions are held as a means of socialization [1]. However, khat has been increasingly used for recreational purposes. It is estimated that between 30 and 50% of Ethiopians are current khat users [6], and between 70–82% of males and 30–43% of females in Yemen use khat [7, 8]. Users typically get together in the afternoon and use khat for several hours [9]. Evidence indicates that chronic khat use is associated with myocardial infarction [10], worsening of ongoing mental illness [11], and impaired neurocognitive functions [12]. Chronic khat use is also related to increased negative affect and attenuated psychobiological responses to stress [13]. Causal directions of these relationships are yet to be determined.

Healthy adults in Yemen who follow the Islamic faith abstain from consumption of food, drinks, substances, as well as sexual intercourse from sunrise to sunset during Ramadan, the ninth month of the lunar calendar. Evidence indicates that khat use is associated with alterations in psychobiological stress responses and subjective mood [13]. There is also evidence that acute fasting is associated with reduced physiological responses to laboratory stress [14], although the impacts of fasting on stress responses and withdrawal experienced by habitual khat users or other drug users are largely unknown. Khat users report depressive and anxious symptoms as well as insomnia after termination of use [15]. These symptoms are commonly reported among users of other substances, such as tobacco [16]. Withdrawal symptoms induce stress, which is a known factor in maintenance of, and relapse to, drug use [17]. The question of whether fasting and khat use status moderate stress responses and withdrawal symptoms is important in addressing the role of stress in khat use.

Here, we report results from a study examining the effects of fasting on stress responses and mood in chronic khat users and nonusers. We used an established protocol to assess subjective and objective markers of stress. Based on previous studies [13, 14], it was predicted that religious fasting (a practice that occurs during Ramadan) would be associated with enhanced negative affect and withdrawal symptoms, as well as attenuated psychophysiological responses to acute laboratory stress among khat users relative to nonusers during the fasting periods.

Participants

This study was part of a larger study that examined biobehavioral mechanisms of long-term khat use [12, 13, 18-20]. Recruitment of participants was completed through flyers posted around Sana’a University in Yemen and surrounding communities. Inclusion criteria were as follows: no current nor recent history of major physical diseases, no psychiatric disorders, and no current use of medications. Individuals who reported khat use for the past 2 years were classified as khat users and those who reported that they had never used khat were classified as nonusers. Previous studies from the same project [12, 13, 18-20] did not include individuals who completed study sessions during the month of Ramadan. This procedure was used to eliminate potential confounding effects of fasting. We used gender and khat use status of individuals who completed sessions while fasting to match them with those who completed sessions while not fasting. As a result, this study included 40 individuals (12 females and 28 males; 25 khat users and 15 nonusers) in each fasting group. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Sana’a University. All participants received monetary incentives for their participation in the study.

Measures and Apparatus

The Subjective State Measure (SSS; [21]) is an 18-item self-report questionnaire that assesses mood states. An Arabic version was developed and adapted in our previous studies [13]. Information about these measures and their use with similar populations has been published elsewhere [13, 20, 22]. Due to some commonalities in symptomatology between khat withdrawal [15] and tobacco withdrawal [16], we measured the severity of khat withdrawal using items that assessed irritability, anger, anxiety, difficulty concentration, restlessness, sadness, and hunger. These items were adapted from the Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale [16, 23]. Systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and heart rate (HR) were collected by a MicroLife Automatic Blood Pressure Monitor BP 3AC1. The oscillometric device has been validated by the European Society of Hypertension’s International protocol. Measurement of expired carbon monoxide was completed using MicroCO monitors (Micro Direct, Auburn, ME, USA).

Procedures

Participants were asked to complete an onsite, semi-structured screening interview for assessment of eligibility, which included questions regarding medical and substance use histories. Qualified participants were asked to schedule 1 ambulatory and 1 laboratory session. A set of guidelines and instructions about diet and sleep was provided prior to those sessions. Each participant was tested individually. On the ambulatory session day, participants reported to the laboratory between 10 a.m. and noon to receive a set of mood questionnaires (e.g., SSS). The participants were instructed to complete the SSS 5 times, at: 10:00 a.m.; 7:00 p.m.; 10:00 p.m., or before going to bed; 8:00 a.m. the next day, immediately after waking up; and 9:00 a.m. This timing was kept consistent across all groups of participants. Participants returned to the laboratory the next morning. All laboratory stress sessions began between 9 a.m. and 10 a.m. Upon arrival, the participants sat in a comfortable chair. After a blood pressure cuff was attached, the participants rested for 30 min. This baseline period was followed by exposure to an acute stress challenge (10 min). The participants were asked to subtract the number 7 or 13 from a four-digit number. When the participants made a mistake, they were asked to go back to the previous correct response. This task has been used in stress reactivity studies [24]. Then, participants rested for another 30 min. Blood pressure and HR were obtained every 5 min during baseline and post-stress recovery, and they were obtained every 3 min during the stress task. SSS was administered at the end of each period.

Data Analysis

Subjective mood data from the ambulatory sessions (SSS positive affect and negative affect) were analyzed using a 2 fasting group (fasting and no-fasting) × 2 khat use group (current user and nonuser) × 5 sampling times (10 a.m., 7 p.m., 10 p.m. or bedtime, 8 a.m. or wake-up, and 9 a.m.) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with Wilks’ lambda test. Withdrawal measures were analyzed in khat users only using a 2 fasting group × 5 sampling times MANOVA with Wilks’ lambda test. For data analysis of laboratory data, cardiovascular measures were averaged in each period. A series of 2 fasting group × 2 khat use group × 3 times (baseline, stress, and recovery) MANOVAs with Wilks’ lambda tests were conducted on cardiovascular (SBP, DBP, and HR) and mood measures. Khat withdrawal symptoms were analyzed in khat users only. If a significant interaction was found, follow-up tests were conducted using the Bonferroni correction. Slight variations in reported degrees of freedom existed due to missing data.

Participant Characteristics

The mean age of the sample was 22.9 years (SD = 5.6), and the mean BMI was 20.6 (SD = 4.0). Reported average hours of nighttime sleep over the past week was 7.2 (SD = 1.8). There were no fasting nor khat use group differences in these measures (see Table 1). On average, khat users started chewing when they were 15.5 years old (SD = 4.3), and they used khat 5.3 days a week (SD = 2.1) for 6.7 years (SD = 5.3). There were no fasting group differences in khat exposure. Thirty percent of khat users (24/80) reported smoking. Smokers were equally distributed in 2 fasting groups (p = nonsignificant; see Table 1); they smoked 14.2 cigarettes per day (SD = 5.3). Smokers in the fasting group smoked for more years than those in the no-fasting group (F[1, 18] = 5.07, p = 0.04). Expired carbon monoxide was lower in smokers who were in the fasting group than in smokers in the no-fasting group (F[1, 22] = 9.08, p = 0.006), which provided support for abstinence from tobacco use in the fasting group.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics as a function of fasting and khat use

Participant characteristics as a function of fasting and khat use
Participant characteristics as a function of fasting and khat use

Ambulatory Study

Khat users had lower positive affect than nonusers, as found by a main effect of the khat use group (F[1, 70] = 9.65, p = 0.003). This main effect was further qualified by a khat use group × time interaction (F[4, 67] = 2.50, p = 0.05). Follow-up MANOVAs conducted in each khat use group (adjusted p value = 0.05/2 = 0.025) indicated that levels at 10 p.m./bedtime were lower than those at 10 a.m. of the same day in the khat users (p < 0.04); however, this was not found in nonusers (see Fig. 1a). Also, a significant group main effect in negative affect indicated greater levels in khat users than in nonusers (F[1, 70] = 12.8, p < 0.001). Negative affect at 8 a.m./waking was lower than that at 10 a.m. the day prior (time main effect: F[4, 67] = 2.77, p = 0.03; see Fig. 1b). There was a fasting group × time interaction in withdrawal symptoms (F[4, 42] = 4.48, p = 0.004). A follow-up MANOVA revealed greater symptoms during the evening hours (7 and 10 p.m.) than during the morning hours the next day (9 a.m.) in the no-fasting group (p ≤ 0.005); however, this time effect was not found in the fasting group (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Changes in positive affect (a), negative affect (b), and withdrawal symptoms (ambulatory study) (c). Entries show mean and bars indicate standard error of the mean. aA khat use group x time interaction (p = 0.05) indicated that positive affect at 10 pm/bedtime was lower than the first sample (10 am) of the same day in khat use group (p < 0.04) only. bKhat users had greater negative affect than nonusers (p < 0.001). c Negative affect levels of 8 am or awake were lower than the first sample (10 am) of the day prior (p = 0.03). dA significant fasting x time interaction in withdrawal symptoms was due to lower levels at 9 am sample than 7 pm and 10 pm samples in the no-fasting group only.

Fig. 1.

Changes in positive affect (a), negative affect (b), and withdrawal symptoms (ambulatory study) (c). Entries show mean and bars indicate standard error of the mean. aA khat use group x time interaction (p = 0.05) indicated that positive affect at 10 pm/bedtime was lower than the first sample (10 am) of the same day in khat use group (p < 0.04) only. bKhat users had greater negative affect than nonusers (p < 0.001). c Negative affect levels of 8 am or awake were lower than the first sample (10 am) of the day prior (p = 0.03). dA significant fasting x time interaction in withdrawal symptoms was due to lower levels at 9 am sample than 7 pm and 10 pm samples in the no-fasting group only.

Close modal

Laboratory Study

Khat users had lower positive affect than nonusers (khat use group main effect: F[1, 70] = 4.57, p = 0.04; see Table 2). A significant main effect of time in positive affect (F[2, 69] = 5.46, p = 0.006) was qualified by a fasting group × time interaction (F[2, 69] = 3.99, p = 0.02). This interaction was due to a stress-related decrease in the no-fasting group (post hoc: p = 0.002); positive affect remained low and flattened in the fasting group (Fig. 2a). With respect to negative affect, khat users had greater negative affect than nonusers (F[1, 70] = 9.88, p = 0.002). A fasting group × time interaction (F[2, 69] = 3.07, p = 0.05) found a stress-related increase in negative affect in the no-fasting group (post hoc: p = 0.009) that was not found in the fasting group (see Fig. 2b). Withdrawal symptoms increased in response to stress in all groups (time effect: F[2, 44] = 3.29, p = 0.047), but this change was not associated with fasting nor khat use.

Table 2.

Changes in self-report mood and cardiovascular measures in the laboratory session

Changes in self-report mood and cardiovascular measures in the laboratory session
Changes in self-report mood and cardiovascular measures in the laboratory session
Fig. 2.

Changes in positive affect (a) and negative affect (b) as a function of fasting (laboratory study). Entries show mean and bars indicate standard error of the mean. aAsignificant fasting group × time interaction in positive affect indicated a stress-related decrease in the no-fasting group only. bA significant fasting group × time interaction in negative affect showed a stress-related increase in no-fasting group only.

Fig. 2.

Changes in positive affect (a) and negative affect (b) as a function of fasting (laboratory study). Entries show mean and bars indicate standard error of the mean. aAsignificant fasting group × time interaction in positive affect indicated a stress-related decrease in the no-fasting group only. bA significant fasting group × time interaction in negative affect showed a stress-related increase in no-fasting group only.

Close modal

There were significant main effects of time in blood pressure (SBP: F[2, 70] = 16.6, p < 0.001; DBP: F[2, 70] = 10.5, p < 0.001) that were further qualified by khat use group × time interactions (SBP: F[2, 70] = 5.37, p = 0.007; DBP: F[2, 70] = 9.20, p < 0.001; see Table 2). These effects indicated stress-related increases in nonusers (SBP: p < 0.001; DBP: p = 0.001) but not in khat users (see Fig. 3a, b). For HR, a significant time effect (F[2, 65] = 7.91, p = 0.001) indicated a stress-related increase in all groups (p = 0.03; see Fig. 3c). There were no differences related to fasting in cardiovascular measures.

Fig. 3.

Changes in SBP (a), DBP (b), and HR (c) as a function of khat use (laboratory study). SBP, systolic blood pressure; diastolic blood pressure; HR, heart rate. Entries show mean and bars indicate standard error of the mean. aA significant khat use group × time interaction indicated a stress-related increase in SBP in nonusers only. bA significant khat use group × time interaction indicated a stress-related increase in DBP in nonusers only. cAll participants showed an increased in heart rate in response to stress.

Fig. 3.

Changes in SBP (a), DBP (b), and HR (c) as a function of khat use (laboratory study). SBP, systolic blood pressure; diastolic blood pressure; HR, heart rate. Entries show mean and bars indicate standard error of the mean. aA significant khat use group × time interaction indicated a stress-related increase in SBP in nonusers only. bA significant khat use group × time interaction indicated a stress-related increase in DBP in nonusers only. cAll participants showed an increased in heart rate in response to stress.

Close modal

This study shows that fasting and habitual khat use independently influence subjective and physiological indices related to stress. Khat users in a no-fasting group reported increased withdrawal symptoms during the evening hours compared to those in the fasting group. The group difference peaked in the late evening and diminished the next morning. This pattern is consistent with previous research that examined withdrawal symptomatology of khat use. Khat users typically get together with peers in the early afternoon and start chewing khat [9], and they usually smoke tobacco during this period [9, 25]. Users report negative affect and insomnia after khat sessions [15]. The current finding of increased withdrawal symptoms during the evening hours among khat users who were not fasting may reflect elevation in post-use symptoms. In contrast, during Ramadan, adults abstain from eating, drinking, and substance use from sunrise to sunset. It is possible that lower withdrawal symptoms in the fasting group reflected full abstinence from substance use, use of substance(s) after sunset, or a mixture of these conditions. Asking about substance use at each time point might improve results; however, we note the possibility that people may not disclose drug use during Ramadan due to social desirability.

Fasting and no-fasting groups showed different response profiles to acute stress. The no-fasting group reported increased negative affect and decreased positive affect in response to stress. These results are consistent with patterns observed in healthy adults without diet restrictions [24]. The causes of the flattened responses in the fasting group are not known at this time, although several factors might have contributed to this observation. First, one purpose of Ramadan is supposedly to enhance awareness of people who are poor and not fortunate through prayer, charity, and fasting. Fasting is also considered important to gain patience and self-control. It is possible that Ramadan modifies psychological processes in response to stress. The other possibility is that fasting changes metabolic functions [26] and leads to changes in central and peripheral systems associated with stress and coping. Fasting did not influence cardiovascular responses to stress; exposure to stress resulted in the expected changes. Future research should elucidate associations between fasting during Ramadan and stress mechanisms through multilevel measures, including stress and appetite hormones.

Chronic khat use was associated with lower positive affect and enhanced negative affect during ambulatory and laboratory sessions, yet khat users exhibited attenuated cardiovascular responses to stress. These findings are consistent with those of prior studies among khat users [13], tobacco users [27], and concurrent khat and tobacco users [20]. Animal studies have also shown that chronic administration of cathinone, one of the main constituents of khat, is related to dose- and time-dependent reductions in serum cortisol. There is a growing body of literature suggesting that fasting induces functional changes in central nervous and endocrine systems [28], both of which play essential roles in stress regulation. To date, there are only 2 human studies that examined the effects of short-term fasting on stress indices, such as cortisol, in controlled environments. One study showed that low levels of glucose after fasting (≥8 h) were associated with attenuated cortisol stress response [14]. Another pilot investigation indicated reduced basal adrenocortical activity among fasting women during the luteal menstrual phase [29]. Results of this study extend existing literature by confirming blunted stress responses among khat users, regardless of fasting. The finding that fasting did not moderate the link between khat use status and stress adds support to the robustness of khat-related dysregulation of the stress response.

Due to a small sample size, results of this study should be considered preliminary. Also, our findings are limited by our use of a cross-sectional approach. A within-subject design (each person completes 1 session while fasting and 1 session while not fasting) may improve results. This study lacked objective measures of fasting and khat use. We did not know whether participants in the fasting group were truly abstinent from food and khat use. However, low CO in the fasting group lends some support for drug abstinence in this group. There is no gold standard questionnaire to assess khat withdrawal. While self-reported symptoms after khat use are similar to those after smoking or drinking, research examining the psychobiological impacts of khat withdrawal is needed. In this study, 1 research staff member administered a mental arithmetic task to measure psychophysiological responses to stress. This approach was based on local vetting, refinement of the protocol, and available human resources. However, future research should consider using a combination of mental math and other challenging tasks, such as public speaking or anger recall interviews, to reliably assess psychobiological stress responses. Biomarkers of stress, including adrenocorticotropic hormone, cortisol, and HR variability, should be included in future research. It should be noted that we did collect saliva samples for the measurement of stress responses; however, we encountered some inconsistencies in labeling samples, and we were not able to carefully analyze them. Nevertheless, the use of a larger sample with sufficient statistical power, multiple stress tasks, and stress measures should improve current results. Strengths of this study include our use of established protocol to assess stress and our use of a matched sample to mitigate the effects of confounding factors on observed findings.

In conclusion, this study is the first to report different patterns of stress responses and withdrawal symptoms between khat users who were fasting and those who were not fasting during laboratory and ambulatory periods. The study also found flattened emotional responses to acute stress in fasting individuals. Results show complex relationships among fasting, khat use, and stress mechanisms. An emerging literature suggests potential protective effects of fasting against chronic diseases [30]. Findings from this study call for the need of researchers to examine the potential effectiveness of fasting, not necessarily in the context of religious fasting, in reducing stimulant use.

We thank the following for their help with this project: Najat Saem Khalil, Molham Al Habori for helping with the data collection; Amal Alanisi, Tawfeek Alharazi, Ashrak alAwdari, Bakeer Dahmash, and Essa Oumairi for assisting with data collection; and Abdul Kareem Nasher for help with data entry.

All study procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Minnesota, and the study was conducted ethically in accordance with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki.

The authors of this manuscript do not have any conflict of interested related to this work.

M.A. designed the study and developed the protocol. M.N. conducted literature review on relevant studies and data management and analysis. M.N. and M.A. interpreted results of the analysis. Both authors contributed to the development of the manuscript.

This research was supported by a FIRCA grant from the National Institutes of Health/Fogarty International Center (R03TW007219), an R21 National Institute for Drug Abuse grant (DA024626), and a grant from the Office of International Programs at the University of Minnesota. These grants had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

1.
Cox
G
,
Rampes
H
.
Adverse effects of khat: a review
.
Adv Psychiatr Treat
.
2003
;
9
(
6
):
456
63
. .
2.
Kalix
P
.
Catha edulis, a plant that has amphetamine effects
.
Pharm World Sci
.
1996
;
18
(
2
):
69
73
. .
3.
Widler
P
,
Mathys
K
,
Brenneisen
R
,
Kalix
P
,
Fisch
HU
.
Pharmacodynamics and pharmacokinetics of khat: a controlled study
.
Clin Pharmacol Ther
.
1994
;
55
(
5
):
556
62
. .
4.
Rothman
RB
,
Vu
N
,
Partilla
JS
,
Roth
BL
,
Hufeisen
SJ
,
Compton-Toth
BA
, et al
In vitro characterization of ephedrine-related stereoisomers at biogenic amine transporters and the receptorome reveals selective actions as norepinephrine transporter substrates
.
J Pharmacol Exp Ther
.
2003
;
307
(
1
):
138
45
. .
5.
Simmler
LD
,
Buser
TA
,
Donzelli
M
,
Schramm
Y
,
Dieu
LH
,
Huwyler
J
, et al
Pharmacological characterization of designer cathinones in vitro
.
Br J Pharmacol
.
2013
;
168
(
2
):
458
70
. .
6.
Alem
A
,
Kebede
D
,
Kullgren
G
.
The prevalence and socio-demographic correlates of khat chewing in Butajira, Ethiopia
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl
.
1999
;
397
:
84
91
. .
7.
Numan
N
.
Exploration of adverse psychological symptoms in yemeni khat users by the symptoms checklist-90 (SCL-90)
.
Addiction
.
2004
;
99
(
1
):
61
5
. .
8.
The World Bank
.
Yemen-towards qat demand reduction
.
Washington, DC
:
World Bank
;
2007
.
9.
Nakajima
M
,
al’Absi
M
,
Dokam
A
,
Alsoofi
M
,
Khalil
NS
,
Al Habori
M
.
Gender differences in patterns and correlates of khat and tobacco use
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2013
;
15
(
6
):
1130
5
. .
10.
Ali
WM
,
Al Habib
KF
,
Al-Motarreb
A
,
Singh
R
,
Hersi
A
,
Al Faleh
H
, et al
Acute coronary syndrome and khat herbal amphetamine use: an observational report
.
Circulation
.
2011
;
124
(
24
):
2681
9
. .
11.
Odenwald
M
,
Neuner
F
,
Schauer
M
,
Elbert
T
,
Catani
C
,
Lingenfelder
B
, et al
Khat use as risk factor for psychotic disorders: a cross-sectional and case-control study in Somalia
.
BMC Med
.
2005
;
3
:
5
. .
12.
Hoffman
R
,
al’Absi
M
.
Working memory and speed of information processing in chronic khat users: preliminary findings
.
Eur Addict Res
.
2013
;
19
(
1
):
1
6
. .
13.
al’Absi
M
,
Khalil
NS
,
Al Habori
M
,
Hoffman
R
,
Fujiwara
K
,
Wittmers
L
.
Effects of chronic khat use on cardiovascular, adrenocortical, and psychological responses to stress in men and women
.
Am J Addict
.
2013
;
22
(
2
):
99
107
.
14.
Kirschbaum
C
,
Gonzalez Bono
E
,
Rohleder
N
,
Gessner
C
,
Pirke
KM
,
Salvador
A
, et al
Effects of fasting and glucose load on free cortisol responses to stress and nicotine
.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab
.
1997
;
82
(
4
):
1101
5
. .
15.
Hassan
NA
,
Gunaid
AA
,
El-Khally
FM
,
Murray-Lyon
IM
.
The effect of chewing Khat leaves on human mood
.
Saudi Med J
.
2002
;
23
(
7
):
850
3
.
16.
Hughes
JR
,
Hatsukami
D
.
Signs and symptoms of tobacco withdrawal
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
1986
;
43
(
3
):
289
94
. .
17.
al’Absi
M
.
Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical responses to psychological stress and risk for smoking relapse
.
Int J Psychophysiol
.
2006
;
59
(
3
):
218
27
. .
18.
Hoffman
R
,
Al’absi
M
.
Concurrent use of khat and tobacco is associated with verbal learning and delayed recall deficits
.
Addiction
.
2013
;
108
(
10
):
1855
62
. .
19.
Nakajima
M
,
Hoffman
R
,
Al’Absi
M
.
Poor working memory and reduced blood pressure levels in concurrent users of khat and tobacco
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2013
;
16
(
3
):
279
87
. .
20.
al’Absi
M
,
Nakajima
M
,
Dokam
A
,
Sameai
A
,
Alsoofi
M
,
Saem Khalil
N
, et al
Concurrent tobacco and khat use is associated with blunted cardiovascular stress response and enhanced negative mood: a cross-sectional investigation
.
Hum Psychopharmacol
.
2014
;
29
(
4
):
307
15
.
21.
al’Absi
M
,
Wittmers
LE
,
Erickson
J
,
Hatsukami
D
,
Crouse
B
.
Attenuated adrenocortical and blood pressure responses to psychological stress in ad libitum and abstinent smokers
.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav
.
2003
;
74
(
2
):
401
10
.
22.
Bongard
S
,
al’Absi
M
,
Khalil
NS
,
Al Habori
M
.
Khat use and trait anger: effects on affect regulation during an acute stressful challenge
.
Eur Addict Res
.
2011
;
17
(
6
):
285
91
. .
23.
Hughes
J
,
Hatsukami
DK
.
Errors in using tobacco withdrawal scale
.
Tob Control
.
1998
;
7
(
1
):
92
3
. .
24.
al’Absi
M
,
Bongard
S
,
Buchanan
T
,
Pincomb
GA
,
Licinio
J
,
Lovallo
WR
.
Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine adjustment to public speaking and mental arithmetic stressors
.
Psychophysiology
.
1997
;
34
(
3
):
266
75
.
25.
Kassim
S
,
Islam
S
,
Croucher
RE
.
Correlates of nicotine dependence in U.K. resident yemeni khat chewers: a cross-sectional study
.
Nicotine Tob Res
.
2011
;
13
(
12
):
1240
9
. .
26.
Mattson
MP
,
Wan
R
.
Beneficial effects of intermittent fasting and caloric restriction on the cardiovascular and cerebrovascular systems
.
J Nutr Biochem
.
2005
;
16
(
3
):
129
37
. .
27.
al’Absi
M
,
Nakajima
M
,
Grabowski
J
.
Stress response dysregulation and stress-induced analgesia in nicotine dependent men and women
.
Biol Psychol
.
2013
;
93
(
1
):
1
8
.
28.
Longo
VD
,
Mattson
MP
.
Fasting: molecular mechanisms and clinical applications
.
Cell Metab
.
2014
;
19
(
2
):
181
92
. .
29.
Ohara
K
,
Okita
Y
,
Kouda
K
,
Mase
T
,
Miyawaki
C
,
Nakamura
H
.
Cardiovascular response to short-term fasting in menstrual phases in young women: an observational study
.
BMC Womens Health
.
2015
;
15
:
67
. .
30.
Mattson
MP
,
Longo
VD
,
Harvie
M
.
Impact of intermittent fasting on health and disease processes
.
Ageing Res Rev
.
2017
;
39
:
46
58
. .
Copyright / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Copyright: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be translated into other languages, reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, microcopying, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.