Introduction: Wearable devices are rapidly improving our ability to observe health-related processes for extended durations in an unintrusive manner. In this study, we use wearable devices to understand how the shape of the heart rate curve during sleep relates to mental health. Methods: As part of the Lived Experiences Measured Using Rings Study (LEMURS), we collected heart rate measurements using the Oura ring (Gen3) for over 25,000 sleep periods and self-reported mental health indicators from roughly 600 first-year university students in the USA during the fall semester of 2022. Using clustering techniques, we find that the sleeping heart rate curves can be broadly separated into two categories that are mainly differentiated by how far along the sleep period the lowest heart rate is reached. Results: Sleep periods characterized by reaching the lowest heart rate later during sleep are also associated with shorter deep and REM sleep and longer light sleep, but not a difference in total sleep duration. Aggregating sleep periods at the individual level, we find that consistently reaching the lowest heart rate later during sleep is a significant predictor of (1) self-reported impairment due to anxiety or depression, (2) a prior mental health diagnosis, and (3) firsthand experience in traumatic events. This association is more pronounced among females. Conclusion: Our results show that the shape of the sleeping heart rate curve, which is only weakly correlated with descriptive statistics such as the average or the minimum heart rate, is a viable but mostly overlooked metric that can help quantify the relationship between sleep and mental health.

Sleep is an important component of well-being, with poor sleep leading to impaired function at the individual and societal [1‒5] levels. While some aspects of sleep can be easily monitored, such as the awakening time or the time to bed, the unconscious nature of sleep requires external monitoring for proper assessment [6]. Polysomnography has been the gold standard for sleep monitoring, but it requires controlled conditions in a clinical setting, making it costly and inconvenient [7, 8]. In comparison, the growing availability of consumer-grade wearable devices allows individuals to monitor their sleep for extended periods in a nondisruptive and more affordable manner without the recall bias characteristic of self-reports [9]. Feedback from wearable devices is also available soon after data collection, which aids in delivering and assessing the effects of behavioral changes such as interventions.

Sleep has mostly been assessed in terms of sleep duration, sleep efficiency, and the time spent in different sleep stages [10, 11]. Heart rate variability (HRV) is also of interest, particularly in studies on sleep apnea [12‒14], and time- and frequency-domain analysis techniques are commonly used [15, 16]. Clustering techniques have been applied on these metrics to find sleep phenotypes, sleep patterns shared by a group of individuals that may also share similar characteristics [17‒20]. While most use descriptive statistics of these metrics in the clustering algorithm, more recent work used features extracted from the raw sleep-wake time series from more than 100,000 individuals to infer sleep phenotypes related to insomnia [21].

While it has long been known that heart rate generally decreases during sleep [22], the patterns of change in the sleeping heart rate are not well-studied. Unlike other sleep metrics such as sleep stages or HRV, heart rate is more reliably measured by consumer wearable devices [23‒27] using photoplethysmography [28], especially when the participant is at rest [29]. Heart rate patterns during sleep may give us valuable insights that may be more consistent across brands of wearable devices compared to those obtained using other sleep metrics.

Mental health has been shown to have a bidirectional relationship with sleep [5, 30, 31], indicating that treating sleep disorders may also improve mental health [32]. Most studies on mental health and sleep focus on perceived sleep quality [33], sleep duration [34‒37], HRV [16, 38], or descriptive statistics (mean or minimum) of the heart rate [39‒42]. Published studies on the relationship between the shape of the heart rate curve and mental health have focused on its periodicity in the context of daily circadian rhythms [43‒46] or its shape for daytime and nighttime [47] but not on how heart rate changes during the sleep period itself.

Young adulthood is a critical life stage in detecting and treating mental health conditions and has been the subject of several studies [48‒51]. An ongoing study at a university in the northeast USA (the Lived Experiences Measured Using Rings Study, or LEMURS) [52‒54] monitors the mental and physical well-being of a cohort of college students using surveys and the Oura ring (Gen3), a wearable sleep and activity tracker [23, 24] that provides heart rate measurements in excellent agreement with those from electrocardiography [27]. With around half of study participants diagnosed with a mental health condition or having experienced two or more traumatic events [55], this dataset provides a unique perspective on the relationship between mental health and sleep for a population at risk.

Using data from the LEMURS study, we look at different patterns of change in the heart rate over a sleep period and relate these to the reported mental health indicators of the participants. Similar to the systematic characterization of sleep phenotypes from sleep-wake measurements using wearable devices [21], we perform clustering algorithms on the heart rate time series to see if these heart rate patterns can be grouped into categories. Associations between these categories and clinically relevant information, such as mental health outcomes, are then examined.

Data

Sleep data were obtained from 603 participants in LEMURS, who, at the time of the study, were first-year students in a university in the USA [52‒54]. Participants were asked to wear the Oura ring (Gen3) during sleep for 8 weeks (Oct–Dec 2022). We examine heart rate time series measured by the ring at 5-min intervals from the start of each detected primary sleep period, yielding 25,800 time series for analysis. The ring also collects sleep period start and end times, the estimated duration of each sleep stage (light, REM, and deep sleep), time spent awake during the sleep period, average and lowest heart rates, the time when the lowest heart rate is obtained, sleep latency, average respiratory rate, average respiratory rate variation, and HRV.

To denoise the heart rate time series and make them comparable for sleep periods of different durations, we use piecewise aggregate approximation (PAA) [56, 57], dividing a time series into n = 30 equally sized segments and taking the mean of each. With a median of 102 data points per sleep period, n = 30 roughly corresponds to 15 min of sleep per segment. Using n = 30 allows us to include even the shortest sleep period (39 data points, or roughly 3.25 h of sleep). As movement during sleep results in poorer heart rate estimates [29], the Oura ring does not provide HR measurements when there is significant movement. This creates missing values in the raw time series. Sleep periods where there is at least one segment with a missing PAA value are disregarded, reducing the number of sleep periods examined to m = 20,167 belonging to N = 599 individuals. The disregarded sleep periods span a similar range of sleep period lengths (online suppl. Fig. S6; for all online suppl. material, see https://doi.org/10.1159/000539487) and are from 587 individuals, indicating that disregarding time series with missing PAA values does not sufficiently discriminate against a few users or sleep period durations.

Participants completed baseline surveys (Table S1) about their demographics (e.g., gender, race), mental health diagnoses [58], impairment due to anxiety or depression, and the number of types of traumatic events experienced firsthand [55] (0, 1, or ≥2). They also completed the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) [59] and Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7) [60] questionnaires at the end of each week throughout the study period. The PSS is designed to assess stress while the GAD-7 is a tool used to screen for generalized anxiety disorder.

The students all entered the university in the same academic year, studied full-time, and were required to be within 18–24 years of age to be part of the study. 96% of the students reported birth years between 2002 and 2004.

Clustering the Time Series

We perform k-means clustering [61] on the m = 20,167 heart rate time series processed with PAA. The optimum number of clusters, k, is determined using consistency metrics. First, we use 30 randomly selected subsets of the data (10% of the total size) to train the model. For each training subset, we perform k-means with 30 different centroid initializations and assign the remaining observations to a given cluster based on the trained model. This results in 900 different k-means runs for each value of k. If the clustering is robust, the time series must be clustered similarly for different runs, resulting in the convergence of the cluster centroids across the different randomizations. Once consistent cluster centroids are found, we standardize the cluster centroids by training the k-means model on the entire dataset for 30 different initializations. We perform another round of k-means on these centroids, resulting in what we call “metaclusters.” Cluster centroids assigned to the same metacluster will be assigned the same cluster label, and this cluster label will be transmitted back to the raw heart rate time series. Each raw heart rate time series will have 30 cluster labels, and the mode of these will be the final cluster label assignment for the time series. While we discuss here the results using Euclidean distance in the clustering algorithm, constrained dynamic time warping (cDTW) produced similar results (online suppl. Fig. S5).

Understanding the Clusters

We use logistic regression to gain insight into the sleep measures that differentiate the clusters. Because the clusters were obtained solely from the shapes of the heart rate curves, we do not add random effects. After performing regression models using each measure separately, we combine the sleep measures in a single model and use stepAIC of the MASS library in R to perform stepwise regression. We check for practical significance by examining the distributions and descriptive statistics of each sleep measure for the clusters found.

Relating Clusters to Mental Health

For each sleep period, we take the cluster label as the outcome variable and the survey responses of the individual as the predictors. Specifically, we look at the individual’s baseline survey responses on prior mental health diagnoses, impairment due to anxiety or depression, traumatic events experienced, gender and race, and their weekly PSS and GAD-7 scores. Since the PSS and GAD-7 scores are obtained weekly while the sleep data are obtained nightly, we only include weeks where a given participant answered the weekly survey and had at least three (3) recorded sleep periods. We also require that this restriction yields at least ten (10) sleep periods for a given participant. These ensure that the weight of a given sleep period to the weekly PSS score is not artificially high and that enough data points are available for the fraction of sleep periods in a given cluster for each individual to be a reasonable measure. We note that these steps yield similar distributions of the demographic variables (online suppl. Fig. S7). This results in a dataset with 15,073 sleep periods from N = 505 participants. Logistic mixed-effects models, with the participant ID and the week number treated as random effects, were used in the analysis.

We then focus on predicting an individual’s mental health indicators (impairment, prior mental health diagnosis, and firsthand experience of traumatic events) from their sleeping heart patterns using logistic regression. All regression models were implemented using the Python package pymer4 [62].

Highly consistent cluster labels were obtained across different centroid initializations or training subsets for k = 2 clusters but not for higher values of k (Fig. 1). Using the full dataset to train the clustering model, 99.96% of the sleep periods had identical cluster assignments after performing k-means 30 times, each with different centroid initializations. Much lower values are obtained for higher values of k (online suppl. Fig. S2, S3). This separation into two groups was further confirmed with pairwise correlation maps (online suppl. Fig. S4) and high Jaccard similarity coefficients of obtained clusters under resampling, a method used to check cluster stability [63].

Fig. 1.

Cluster consistency across different training subsets and initializations. Each curve shows a cluster centroid found for a given number of clusters k using a randomized 10% subset of the data and a randomized centroid initialization to run k-means. With 30 different randomized subsets and 30 different centroid initializations, there are 900*k curves in each plot. Highly consistent cluster centroids are found for k = 2 but are not found for k = 3 and higher (see online suppl. Fig. S1 for k > 3).

Fig. 1.

Cluster consistency across different training subsets and initializations. Each curve shows a cluster centroid found for a given number of clusters k using a randomized 10% subset of the data and a randomized centroid initialization to run k-means. With 30 different randomized subsets and 30 different centroid initializations, there are 900*k curves in each plot. Highly consistent cluster centroids are found for k = 2 but are not found for k = 3 and higher (see online suppl. Fig. S1 for k > 3).

Close modal

Cluster Characteristics

Logistic regression reveals that the two clusters are most differentiated by how far along the sleep period the lowest HR is measured. It is the most relevant predictor in the regression model, resulting in the lowest AIC and residual deviance when used as a lone predictor, and is the most highly correlated with the cluster label. We can see this clearly in the distributions of this variable for the two clusters, as well as from the shape of the cluster centroids (Fig. 2a). The time required to reach the lowest heart rate is only weakly correlated with the average heart rate (Spearman’s ρ = 0.1 for both raw time and fraction of the sleep period) and the lowest heart rate (Spearman’s ρ = 0.03 for raw time and ρ = 0.05 for fraction of the sleep period). We denote the cluster with the longer median time to reach the lowest heart rate as cluster 1, and the other cluster as cluster 2, corresponding to 64% and 36% of the sleep periods examined, respectively.

Fig. 2.

Characterizing clusters of sleeping heart rate curves. The two clusters found are most clearly differentiated by the how far along the sleep period the lowest heart rate is reached (a) as compared to other sleep measures taken by the Oura ring. This is consistent with the centroids of the time series in each cluster (inset). Similar behavior is found for the time in hours needed to reach the lowest heart rate (b). The clusters also differ in their sleep stage compositions (c–e) but not in the total sleep duration (f). Smaller but statistically significant differences are also observed for the average heart rate and the average HRV (f–h). Vertical lines indicate the medians for each distribution.

Fig. 2.

Characterizing clusters of sleeping heart rate curves. The two clusters found are most clearly differentiated by the how far along the sleep period the lowest heart rate is reached (a) as compared to other sleep measures taken by the Oura ring. This is consistent with the centroids of the time series in each cluster (inset). Similar behavior is found for the time in hours needed to reach the lowest heart rate (b). The clusters also differ in their sleep stage compositions (c–e) but not in the total sleep duration (f). Smaller but statistically significant differences are also observed for the average heart rate and the average HRV (f–h). Vertical lines indicate the medians for each distribution.

Close modal

The two clusters also differ in terms of sleep stage composition. Cluster 1 is associated with shorter durations of deep (median percentage of the sleep period, 30 vs. 34%) and REM (20 vs. 22%) sleep and longer durations of light sleep (49 vs. 44%). There is no obvious difference in the distributions of the total sleep duration between the two clusters, which is confirmed by the Mann-Whitney U test (p = 0.18, Fig. 2f). Sleep periods in cluster 1 are also characterized by slightly longer sleep latency, earlier bedtime start and earlier bedtime end, higher average heart rate and lower average HRV. As the average HRV is highly negatively correlated with the average heart rate (online suppl. Table S3), we do not include it in the regression model (online suppl. Table S4). While the average respiratory rate variability was statistically significant in the regression model, we did not find this difference between the two clusters to be practically significant. Differences in the means and medians of the sleep metrics between the two clusters are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics of the sleep measures for each cluster

Sleep measure/clusterMeanSDMedianIQR
12121212
Lowest HR time offset (% of sleep period) 64.9 35.1 20.3 26.6 66.0 33.6 29.9 45.4 
% of sleep period in deep sleep 30.5 33.7 11.8 11.5 30.4 33.6 16.0 15.7 
% of sleep period in REM sleep 20.1 21.8 8.3 8.6 20.2 21.6 10.8 11.0 
% of sleep period in light sleep 49.4 44.5 10.8 10.6 49.1 44.1 14.9 14.8 
Total sleep duration, h 7.49 7.46 1.44 1.37 7.48 7.43 1.77 1.71 
Sleep efficiency, % 87.8 88.5 5.5 5.2 89.0 89.0 7.0 6.0 
Sleep latency, min 12.9 9.0 12.6 6.6 9.0 7.0 11.5 8.0 
Bedtime start, h (from 12 mn) 0.46 0.63 1.67 1.61 0.38 0.54 1.99 1.97 
Bedtime end, h (from 12 mn) 9.01 9.06 1.76 1.67 8.92 8.98 2.21 2.12 
Bedtime duration, h 8.54 8.44 1.60 1.54 8.52 8.43 1.92 1.88 
Average respiratory rate, breaths/min 15.6 15.5 1.7 1.6 15.5 15.4 2.4 2.1 
Average respiratory rate variability, breaths/min 3.35 3.36 0.68 0.70 3.25 3.25 0.875 0.875 
Average heart rate, beats/min 63.9 62.2 9.2 9.1 63.6 61.8 12.6 12.4 
Average HRV, ms 63.3 67.2 32.1 33.6 58.0 62.0 43.0 42.0 
Sleep measure/clusterMeanSDMedianIQR
12121212
Lowest HR time offset (% of sleep period) 64.9 35.1 20.3 26.6 66.0 33.6 29.9 45.4 
% of sleep period in deep sleep 30.5 33.7 11.8 11.5 30.4 33.6 16.0 15.7 
% of sleep period in REM sleep 20.1 21.8 8.3 8.6 20.2 21.6 10.8 11.0 
% of sleep period in light sleep 49.4 44.5 10.8 10.6 49.1 44.1 14.9 14.8 
Total sleep duration, h 7.49 7.46 1.44 1.37 7.48 7.43 1.77 1.71 
Sleep efficiency, % 87.8 88.5 5.5 5.2 89.0 89.0 7.0 6.0 
Sleep latency, min 12.9 9.0 12.6 6.6 9.0 7.0 11.5 8.0 
Bedtime start, h (from 12 mn) 0.46 0.63 1.67 1.61 0.38 0.54 1.99 1.97 
Bedtime end, h (from 12 mn) 9.01 9.06 1.76 1.67 8.92 8.98 2.21 2.12 
Bedtime duration, h 8.54 8.44 1.60 1.54 8.52 8.43 1.92 1.88 
Average respiratory rate, breaths/min 15.6 15.5 1.7 1.6 15.5 15.4 2.4 2.1 
Average respiratory rate variability, breaths/min 3.35 3.36 0.68 0.70 3.25 3.25 0.875 0.875 
Average heart rate, beats/min 63.9 62.2 9.2 9.1 63.6 61.8 12.6 12.4 
Average HRV, ms 63.3 67.2 32.1 33.6 58.0 62.0 43.0 42.0 

Mental Health and Sleeping Heart Rate Curves

Logistic mixed-effects regression is used to reveal relationships between the cluster membership of a given sleep period and the associated individual’s demographic and mental health indicators. For this analysis, cluster label is the response and participant ID is a random effect. Adding the week number as either a random or a fixed effect, or excluding it entirely, resulted in similar coefficients and p values for the other predictors (Table 2 vs. online suppl. Table S5). We thus omit the week number in our final models.

Table 2.

Results of logistic mixed-effects regression models for predicting the cluster membership of a sleep period using variables related to the individual, with the participant ID as a random effect

Fixed effectsModel with lone predictorsFinal model
odds ratio2.5% CI97.5% CIp valueodds ratio2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
Gender (male) 1.207 0.940 1.552 0.141 1.324 1.031 1.701 0.028 
Gender (nonbinary) 1.345 0.857 2.111 0.198 1.205 0.771 1.883 0.413 
Race (non-white) 0.942 0.680 1.304 0.718     
Impairment due to anxiety/depression 1.538 1.226 1.929 <0.001 1.477 1.163 1.875 0.001 
Prior mental health diagnosis 1.435 1.156 1.782 0.001     
Traumatic events experienced (≥2) 1.453 1.167 1.809 <0.001 1.329 1.061 1.664 0.013 
Fixed effectsModel with lone predictorsFinal model
odds ratio2.5% CI97.5% CIp valueodds ratio2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
Gender (male) 1.207 0.940 1.552 0.141 1.324 1.031 1.701 0.028 
Gender (nonbinary) 1.345 0.857 2.111 0.198 1.205 0.771 1.883 0.413 
Race (non-white) 0.942 0.680 1.304 0.718     
Impairment due to anxiety/depression 1.538 1.226 1.929 <0.001 1.477 1.163 1.875 0.001 
Prior mental health diagnosis 1.435 1.156 1.782 0.001     
Traumatic events experienced (≥2) 1.453 1.167 1.809 <0.001 1.329 1.061 1.664 0.013 
Coeff2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
Week number 0.013 −0.005 0.031 0.15     
PSS −0.003 −0.011 0.005 0.493     
GAD −0.004 −0.016 0.008 0.52     
Coeff2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
Week number 0.013 −0.005 0.031 0.15     
PSS −0.003 −0.011 0.005 0.493     
GAD −0.004 −0.016 0.008 0.52     

For the dependent variable, cluster 1 is coded as 1 and cluster 2 is coded as 0.

The following predictors were considered fixed effects (Table S1): gender (68% female, 26% male, 6% other genders), race (88% white, 12% non-white), PSS scores, GAD-7 scores, existence of a prior mental health diagnosis (45% with, 55% without), the number of traumatic event categories experienced firsthand [54] (28% with 0, 32% with 1, 40% with ≥2), and impairment due to anxiety or depression (35% with, 65% without). We note that 96% of those with a prior mental health diagnosis reported anxiety or depression.

While prior mental health diagnosis and impairment are highly correlated (Spearman’s ρ = 0.8) and should not be used in the same model, we can test which of these two predictors produces a better fit. Similarly, PSS and GAD-7 scores are highly correlated (Spearman’s ρ = 0.7) so we only use one in any given model to determine the effect of stress or anxiety levels.

Having a prior mental health diagnosis, impairment, or firsthand experience in 2 or more traumatic events are highly significant (p ≤ 0.001) as lone predictors of the sleep period cluster. Gender is not statistically significant at α = 0.05 when used as a lone predictor or used together with traumatic events experienced but is statistically significant when used together with impairment or prior mental health diagnoses (Table 2). PSS scores, GAD-7 scores, and race are not statistically significant at α = 0.05, regardless of whether they are used as lone predictors or used in conjunction with others. Interaction effects between gender and impairment, as well as gender and traumatic events experienced, were not statistically significant when the main effects are also included.

For the final model (Table 2), we use impairment, traumatic events experienced, and gender as fixed effects. Impairment was highly associated with being in cluster 1, which is characterized by reaching the lowest HR later during sleep, as is having experienced 2 or more traumatic events. Being male is also associated with higher odds of being in cluster 1.

As a post hoc analysis (online suppl. Table S6), we also ran the regression models separately for males, females, and those who do not identify as either gender, which we denote here as “non-binary.” Impairment is associated with increased odds of being in cluster 1 in sleep periods among females, while the number of traumatic events (whether <2 or ≥2) is associated with increased odds of being in cluster 1 in sleep periods among females and nonbinary individuals. Both variables are not statistically significant for males. In addition, if we restrict the data to sleep periods of those with or without impairment, we find that the odds of being in cluster 1 among those without impairment are higher for males than for females. No gender effect is seen among those with impairment. On the other hand, gender is only a significant predictor for those with two or more traumatic events, with nonbinary individuals having sleep periods of higher odds of being in cluster 1, but not for those who experienced fewer than two.

With both the normalized and raw time to reach the lowest heart rate being main differentiators of the two clusters (Fig. 2a, b), we look at whether these variables are also associated with demographic information. Using linear mixed-effects regression with the participant ID as a random effect (online suppl. Table S7), we see that impairment continues to be a significant predictor for both the normalized time (p = 0.010) and raw time (p = 0.015). However, while gender (p = 0.001) and traumatic events (p = 0.021) are also significant predictors for normalized time, similar to what we observed for the cluster prediction, they are not significant in predicting raw time (p > 0.05). This indicates that while different genders with similar mental health information may reach the lowest HR at similar times, the difference in the shape of the heart rate curve can be attributed to the shorter sleep duration among males (online suppl. Fig. S9; online suppl. Table S9). On the other hand, the relationship between mental health and the time to reach the lowest heart rate is robust.

Predicting an Individual’s Mental Health Indicator from Sleep Period Cluster Data

With the main predictors not related to any of the weekly measures, we can aggregate our data at the individual level and predict mental health indicators from how often their sleep periods belong in a given cluster. Using logistic regression (Table 3), we find that having a higher fraction of sleep periods in cluster 1 is associated with higher odds of having impairment, a prior mental health diagnosis, or experience in 2 or more traumatic events. It is a statistically significant predictor of being female (vs. non-female). Still, this fraction is not significant in differentiating males and non-males or nonbinary individuals and those who are either male or female.

Table 3.

Results of logistic regression models predicting the response variable from the fraction of sleep periods an individual has in cluster 1

ResponseCoeff2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
With impairment 1.408 0.582 2.235 <0.001 
With mental health diagnosis 1.177 0.404 1.949 0.003 
With ≥2 traumatic events 1.308 0.513 2.103 0.001 
Gender (0 if male, 1 otherwise) −0.705 −1.583 0.172 0.115 
Gender (0 if female, 1 otherwise) 0.840 0.018 1.663 0.045 
Gender (0 if nonbinary, 1 otherwise) −0.832 −2.440 0.776 0.31 
ResponseCoeff2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
With impairment 1.408 0.582 2.235 <0.001 
With mental health diagnosis 1.177 0.404 1.949 0.003 
With ≥2 traumatic events 1.308 0.513 2.103 0.001 
Gender (0 if male, 1 otherwise) −0.705 −1.583 0.172 0.115 
Gender (0 if female, 1 otherwise) 0.840 0.018 1.663 0.045 
Gender (0 if nonbinary, 1 otherwise) −0.832 −2.440 0.776 0.31 

As gender is common demographic information to ask in health-related apps, we check whether this aids in predicting mental health indicators when used together with the fraction of sleep periods in cluster 1. This fraction and gender are statistically significant in predicting impairment and prior mental health diagnoses. However, in predicting experience in 2 or more traumatic events, gender is not statistically significant (see Table S8 for the detailed regression results).

We also perform a post hoc analysis to dig deeper into the effect of gender (Table 4). The fraction of sleep periods belonging to cluster 1 is a statistically significant predictor for impairment only for females; for traumatic events, it is statistically significant only for females and nonbinary individuals. In these cases, having more sleep periods in cluster 1 results in higher odds of having an impairment or experience in two or more traumatic events. This mirrors our earlier post hoc analysis in predicting the sleep period cluster based on the individual’s characteristics. One can get an intuition of the regression results from the composition of our data as given in Table 5 and online supplementary Figures S8, S9, and S10.

Table 4.

Post hoc analysis for predicting the response variable using the fraction of sleep periods in cluster 1 for a given individual in a subset of the data defined by gender

SubsetResponseCoeff2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
Female only With impairment 1.556 0.605 2.506 0.001 
Female only With ≥2 traumatic events 1.166 0.247 2.085 0.013 
Male only With impairment 0.733 −1.553 3.017 0.530 
Male only With ≥2 traumatic events 1.236 −0.604 3.076 0.188 
Nonbinary only With impairment 2.803 −0.371 5.977 0.083 
Nonbinary only With ≥2 traumatic events 3.835 0.154 7.517 0.041 
SubsetResponseCoeff2.5% CI97.5% CIp value
Female only With impairment 1.556 0.605 2.506 0.001 
Female only With ≥2 traumatic events 1.166 0.247 2.085 0.013 
Male only With impairment 0.733 −1.553 3.017 0.530 
Male only With ≥2 traumatic events 1.236 −0.604 3.076 0.188 
Nonbinary only With impairment 2.803 −0.371 5.977 0.083 
Nonbinary only With ≥2 traumatic events 3.835 0.154 7.517 0.041 
Table 5.

Impairment, trauma, and cluster consistency by gender

GenderImpairmentIndividualsNightsfrac_1TraumaIndividualsNightsfrac_1
sizepct (%)sizepct (%)mean (SD)median (IQR)sizepct (%)sizepct (%)mean (SD)median (IQR)
Female (68%) 212 61.6 6,584 62.9 0.59 (0.24) 0.62 (0.37) <204 59.3 6,443 61.5 0.60 (0.25) 0.62 (0.40) 
 132 38.4 3,885 37.1 0.68 (0.24) 0.72 (0.36) ≥2 140 40.7 4,026 38.5 0.66 (0.23) 0.69 (0.30) 
Male (26%) 106 82.2 3,027 84.1 0.66 (0.20) 0.68 (0.25) <83 64.3 2,359 65.5 0.65 (0.21) 0.67 (0.29) 
 23 17.8 574 16.0 0.69 (0.24) 0.72 (0.33) ≥2 46 35.7 1,242 34.5 0.70 (0.18) 0.71 (0.22) 
Nonbinary (6%) 11 34.4 388 38.7 0.57 (0.29) 0.66 (0.44) <18 56.3 584 58.2 0.60 (0.24) 0.65 (0.35) 
 21 65.6 615 61.3 0.74 (0.21) 0.76 (0.28) ≥2 14 43.8 419 41.8 0.79 (0.23) 0.88 (0.25) 
GenderImpairmentIndividualsNightsfrac_1TraumaIndividualsNightsfrac_1
sizepct (%)sizepct (%)mean (SD)median (IQR)sizepct (%)sizepct (%)mean (SD)median (IQR)
Female (68%) 212 61.6 6,584 62.9 0.59 (0.24) 0.62 (0.37) <204 59.3 6,443 61.5 0.60 (0.25) 0.62 (0.40) 
 132 38.4 3,885 37.1 0.68 (0.24) 0.72 (0.36) ≥2 140 40.7 4,026 38.5 0.66 (0.23) 0.69 (0.30) 
Male (26%) 106 82.2 3,027 84.1 0.66 (0.20) 0.68 (0.25) <83 64.3 2,359 65.5 0.65 (0.21) 0.67 (0.29) 
 23 17.8 574 16.0 0.69 (0.24) 0.72 (0.33) ≥2 46 35.7 1,242 34.5 0.70 (0.18) 0.71 (0.22) 
Nonbinary (6%) 11 34.4 388 38.7 0.57 (0.29) 0.66 (0.44) <18 56.3 584 58.2 0.60 (0.24) 0.65 (0.35) 
 21 65.6 615 61.3 0.74 (0.21) 0.76 (0.28) ≥2 14 43.8 419 41.8 0.79 (0.23) 0.88 (0.25) 

frac_1 is the fraction of sleep periods in cluster 1.

Using wearable and self-reported health data from first-year university students, we study how heart rate changes over a sleep period and how these relate to mental health. We find two broad categories of sleeping heart rate curves mainly differentiated by when the lowest heart rate is attained. Sleep periods where the lowest heart rate is reached later are also characterized by shorter deep and REM sleep and longer light sleep, but not significantly different sleep duration. While studies tend to aggregate heart rate measurements into a single statistic (e.g., mean, minimum), these are only weakly correlated with the pattern of change in the sleeping heart rate, indicating that the shape of the sleeping heart rate curve contains new information outside of these aggregate values.

To our knowledge, the only other study on the shapes of sleeping heart rate curves was performed by Oura [64]. They found four shapes, contrasting with the two broad categories for which we found strong support. While the methodology and analysis in this study were not released, we were able to reproduce these four shapes by setting k = 4 in the k-means algorithm and taking the cluster centroids (online suppl. Fig. S3). However, for our dataset, setting k = 4 results in less consistent cluster labels with different training subsets or initializations, unlike the case of k = 2. Our analysis points to a spectrum of shapes, including the four shapes that Oura found that can be broadly categorized into two groups. It is worth noting that Oura’s analysis reflects several orders of magnitude more individuals, with far broader demographic variation.

We also find that these sleeping heart rate patterns are related to mental health indicators, particularly traumatic events experienced and impairment due to anxiety or depression, with the latter being highly correlated with having a prior mental health diagnosis. Individuals who self-report impairment or who have experienced two or more traumatic events [55] are more likely to attain the lowest heart rate later in sleep. The fraction of sleep periods of a participant in a given pattern is a significant predictor for impairment and the number of traumatic events experienced. Finding differential sleep patterns by mental health status is consistent with previous works [65‒67]. With the majority of the mental health diagnoses in our sample being anxiety or depression, our findings also point to links between these disorders and how heart rate changes during sleep.

While several studies show that concurrent stress and anxiety levels affect sleep [40, 68‒70], we find that weekly stress and anxiety scores are not statistically significant in predicting the sleeping heart rate curve pattern. This may be due to a difference in temporal resolution: sleep periods are monitored nightly, while the stress and anxiety levels are only obtained from a weekly self-report [53]. We are implementing more frequent surveys in future studies to ascertain whether daily fluctuations in stress or anxiety relate to how heart rate changes during sleep. Another possible explanation is that the heart rate signatures may have been formed after impairment or trauma and may have stabilized since. These signatures may not be as sensitive to weekly or daily fluctuations in stress or anxiety. More studies will have to be done to test this hypothesis.

We also observe differences across genders in how sleeping heart rate patterns relate to mental health indicators. Impairment and traumatic events experienced are both related to the sleeping heart rate clusters for females, but not for males. For nonbinary individuals, the sleeping heart rate patterns differ by experienced trauma only. Further, males, whether or not they have impairment or traumatic experiences, have higher odds of reaching the lowest heart rate later in sleep. Gender differences in sleep have been observed in prior studies [71‒74], although there are conflicting results [75] possibly due to different study designs. We also note that our sample is highly homogeneous in age, which is not the case in most research on gender differences in sleep. In summary, our results show that not only do we find different patterns in how the heart rate changes during sleep, but that these patterns are related to mental health, indicating that the shape of the heart rate curve is a viable but underexplored sleep metric.

We are interested in studying university students [76] as they are a population at risk [77] for which mental health interventions are highly relevant and more easily implementable [52‒54]. However, this sample limits the generalizability of our results in terms of demographics, sleep disorder prevalence [78], and life stressors [79]. We hope to address these limitations by expanding our recruitment to include a more diverse set of individuals in future work.

The manuscript benefited from helpful discussions with Alec Beauregard, Marco Altini, and Miguel Fudolig. A preprint version of this work is available on PsyArXiv. Mikaela Irene Fudolig, et al. The two fundamental shapes of sleep heart rate dynamics and their connection to mental health. PsyArxiv: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3cqdn (October 13, 2023).

Participants were required to complete a comprehension assessment with completely correct answers before being able to provide written informed consent through REDCap, a HIPAA-compliant online application. This study protocol was reviewed and approved by the University of Vermont Institutional Review Board (protocol number 00002126).

All authors declare no financial or nonfinancial competing interests.

This study was funded by a grant from the MassMutual Financial Group. MassMutual played no role in study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation of data, or the writing of this manuscript. This work was funded in part by the US NIH (MH123031, PI: EM).

M.I.F., L.S.P.B., M.P., J.u.L., E.M., R.M., T.R., P.S.D., and C.D. designed the study. M.I.F., M.P., L.S.P.B., and E.M. performed the analysis. Y.M.B., J.E.H., J.K., J.o.L., and K.S. performed the data collection and cleaning. M.I.F. wrote the manuscript with inputs from all the authors. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

The data to replicate most of the results are available through this public repository: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25877302. Due to the small size of minorities from which the identity of a participant may be inferred, information on the race of the participants will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author. We note that race was not found to be relevant in our results.

1.
Magnavita
N
,
Garbarino
S
.
Sleep, health and wellness at work: a scoping review
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2017
;
14
(
11
):
1347
.
2.
Hafner
M
,
Stepanek
M
,
Taylor
J
,
Troxel
WM
.
Why Sleep Matters-The Economic Costs of Insufficient Sleep: A Cross-Country Comparative Analysis
.
Rand Health Q
.
2017
;
6
(
4
):
11
.
3.
Hillman
D
,
Mitchell
S
,
Streatfeild
J
,
Burns
C
,
Bruck
D
,
Pezzullo
L
.
The economic cost of inadequate sleep
.
Sleep
.
2018
;
41
(
8
):
2
.
4.
Chaput
JP
,
Carrier
J
,
Bastien
C
,
Gari´epy
G
,
Janssen
I
.
Economic burden of insufficient sleep duration in Canadian adults
.
Sleep Health
.
2022
;
8
(3):
298
302
.
5.
Palagini
L
,
Hertenstein
E
,
Riemann
D
,
Nissen
C
.
Sleep, insomnia and mental health
.
J Sleep Res
.
2022
;
31
(
4
):
e13628
.
6.
Ravichandran
R
,
Sien
S-W
,
Patel
SN
,
Kientz
JA
,
Pina
LR
.
Making sense of sleep sensors: how sleep sensing technologies support and undermine sleep health
.
Proceedings of the 2017 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems
.
New York
:
CHI ’17 (Association for Computing Machinery
;
2017
. p.
6864
75
.
7.
Kelly
JM
,
Strecker
RE
,
Bianchi
MT
.
Recent developments in home sleep-monitoring devices
.
ISRN Neurol
.
2012
;
2012
:
e768794
.
8.
Guillodo
E
,
Lemey
C
,
Simonnet
M
,
Walter
M
,
Baca-García
E
,
Masetti
V
,
Moga
S
,
Larsen
M
,
HUGOPSY Network
,
Ropars
J
,
Berrouiguet
S
.
Clinical applications of mobile health wearable–based sleep monitoring: systematic review
.
JMIR Mhealth Uhealth
.
2020
;
8
(
4
):
e10733
.
9.
Lauderdale
DS
,
Knutson
KL
,
Yan
LL
,
Liu
K
,
Rathouz
PJ
.
Sleep duration: how well do self-reports reflect objective measures? The CARDIA Sleep Study
.
Epidemiology
.
2008
;
19
(
6
):
838
45
.
10.
Shrivastava
D
,
Jung
S
,
Saadat
M
,
Sirohi
R
,
Crewson
K
.
How to interpret the results of a sleep study
.
J Community Hosp Intern Med Perspect
.
2014
;
4
(
5
):
24983
.
11.
Kaplan
KA
,
Hirshman
J
,
Hernandez
B
,
Stefanick
ML
,
Hoffman
AR
,
Redline
S
, et al
.
When a gold standard isn’t so golden: lack of prediction of subjective sleep quality from sleep polysomnography
.
Biol Psychol
.
2017
;
123
:
37
46
.
12.
Guilleminault
C
,
Winkle
R
,
Connolly
S
,
Melvin
K
,
Tilkian
A
.
Cyclical variation of the heart rate in sleep apnoea syndrome: mechanisms, and usefulness of 24 h electrocardiography as a screenting technique
.
Lancet
.
1984
;
323
(
8369
):
126
31
.
13.
Sequeira
VCC
,
Bandeira
PM
,
Azevedo
JCM
.
Heart rate variability in adults with obstructive sleep apnea: a systematic review
.
Sleep Sci
.
2019
;
12
(
3
):
214
21
.
14.
Ucak
S
,
Dissanayake
HU
,
Sutherland
K
,
de Chazal
P
,
Cistulli
PA
.
Heart rate variability and obstructive sleep apnea: current perspectives and novel technologies
.
J Sleep Res
.
2021
;
30
(
4
):
e13274
.
15.
Penzel
T
,
Kantelhardt
JW
,
Bartsch
RP
,
Riedl
M
,
Kraemer
JF
,
Wessel
N
, et al
.
Modulations of heart rate, ECG, and cardio-respiratory coupling observed in polysomnography
.
Front Physiol
.
2016
;
7
:
460
.
16.
Correia
ATL
,
Lipinska
G
,
Rauch
HGL
,
Forshaw
PE
,
Roden
LC
,
Rae
DE
.
Associations between sleep-related heart rate variability and both sleep and symptoms of depression and anxiety: a systematic review
.
Sleep Med
.
2023
;
101
:
106
17
.
17.
Miller
CB
,
Bartlett
DJ
,
Mullins
AE
,
Dodds
KL
,
Gordon
CJ
,
Kyle
SD
, et al
.
Clusters of insomnia disorder: an exploratory cluster analysis of objective sleep parameters reveals differences in neurocognitive functioning, quantitative EEG, and heart rate variability
.
Sleep
.
2016
;
39
(
11
):
1993
2004
.
18.
Chiu
VW
,
Ree
M
,
Janca
A
,
Iyyalol
R
,
Dragovic
M
,
Waters
F
.
Sleep profiles and CBT-I response in schizophrenia and related psychoses
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2018
;
268
:
279
87
.
19.
Zinchuk
AV
,
Jeon
S
,
Koo
BB
,
Yan
X
,
Bravata
DM
,
Qin
L
, et al
.
Polysomnographic phenotypes and their cardiovascular implications in obstructive sleep apnoea
.
Thorax
.
2018
;
73
(
5
):
472
80
.
20.
Kramer Fiala Machado
A
,
Wendt
A
,
Menezes
AMB
,
Gonçalves
H
,
Wehrmeister
FC
.
Sleep clusters and modifiable risk behaviors for noncommunicable diseases in young adults: data from a birth cohort in Brazil
.
Sleep Health
.
2023
;
9
(
3
):
346
53
.
21.
Katori
M
,
Shi
S
,
Ode
KL
,
Tomita
Y
,
Ueda
HR
.
The 103,200-arm acceleration dataset in the UK Biobank revealed a landscape of human sleep phenotypes
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2022
;
119
(
12
):
e2116729119
.
22.
Snyder
F
,
Allan Hobson
J
,
Morrison
DF
,
Goldfrank
F
.
Changes in respiration, heart rate, and systolic blood pressure in human sleep
.
J Appl Physiol
.
1964
;
19
:
417
22
.
23.
de Zambotti
M
,
Rosas
L
,
Colrain
IM
,
Baker
FC
.
The sleep of the ring: comparison of the OURA sleep tracker against polysomnography
.
Behav Sleep Med
.
2019
;
17
(
2
):
124
36
.
24.
Altini
M
,
Kinnunen
H
.
The promise of sleep: a multi-sensor approach for accurate sleep stage detection using the Oura ring
.
Sensors
.
2021
;
21
(
13
):
4302
.
25.
Bellenger
CR
,
Miller
DJ
,
Halson
SL
,
Roach
GD
,
Sargent
C
.
Wrist-based photoplethysmography assessment of heart rate and heart rate variability: validation of WHOOP
.
Sensors
.
2021
;
21
(
10
):
3571
.
26.
Imtiaz
SA
.
A systematic review of sensing technologies for wearable sleep staging
.
Sensors
.
2021
;
21
(
5
):
1562
.
27.
Cao
R
,
Azimi
I
,
Sarhaddi
F
,
Niela-Vilen
H
,
Axelin
A
,
Liljeberg
P
, et al
.
Accuracy assessment of Oura Ring nocturnal heart rate and heart rate variability in comparison with electrocardiography in time and frequency domains: comprehensive analysis
.
J Med Internet Res
.
2022
;
24
(
1
):
e27487
.
28.
Castaneda
D
,
Esparza
A
,
Ghamari
M
,
Soltanpur
C
,
Nazeran
H
.
A review on wearable photoplethysmography sensors and their potential future applications in health care
.
Int J Biosens Bioelectron
.
2018
;
4
:
195
202
.
29.
Bent
B
,
Goldstein
BA
,
Kibbe
WA
,
Dunn
JP
.
Investigating sources of inaccuracy in wearable optical heart rate sensors
.
NPJ Digit Med
.
2020
;
3
:
18
9
.
30.
Robotham
D
.
Sleep as a public health concern: insomnia and mental health
.
J Public Ment Health
.
2011
;
10
(
4
):
234
7
.
31.
Sun
X
,
Liu
B
,
Liu
S
,
Wu
DJH
,
Wang
J
,
Qian
Y
, et al
.
Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: a bidirectional Mendelian randomisation study
.
Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci
.
2022
;
31
:
e26
.
32.
Freeman
D
,
Sheaves
B
,
Waite
F
,
Harvey
AG
,
Harrison
PJ
.
Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders
.
Lancet Psychiatr
.
2020
;
7
:
628
37
.
33.
Ramsawh
HJ
,
Stein
MB
,
Belik
S-L
,
Jacobi
F
,
Sareen
J
.
Relationship of anxiety disorders, sleep quality, and functional impairment in a community sample
.
J Psychiatr Res
.
2009
;
43
(
10
):
926
33
.
34.
Rosa
RR
,
Bonnet
MH
,
Kramer
M
.
The relationship of sleep and anxiety in anxious subjects
.
Biol Psychol
.
1983
;
16
(
1–2
):
119
26
.
35.
Fuller
KH
,
Waters
WF
,
Binks
PG
,
Anderson
T
.
Generalized anxiety and sleep architecture: a polysomnographic investigation
.
Sleep
.
1997
;
20
(
5
):
370
6
.
36.
Van Mill
JG
,
Hoogendijk
WJG
,
Vogelzangs
N
,
Van Dyck
R
,
Penninx
BWJH
.
Insomnia and sleep duration in a large cohort of patients with major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders
.
J Clin Psychiatry
.
2010
;
71
(
3
):
239
46
.
37.
Baglioni
C
,
Nanovska
S
,
Regen
W
,
Spiegelhalder
K
,
Feige
B
,
Nissen
C
, et al
.
Sleep and mental disorders: a meta-analysis of polysomnographic research
.
Psychol Bull
.
2016
;
142
(
9
):
969
90
.
38.
Valenza
G
,
Citi
L
,
Lanata
A
,
Gentili
C
,
Barbieri
R
,
Scilingo
EP
.
Applications of heartbeat complexity analysis to depression and bipolar disorder
. In:
Barbieri
R
,
Scilingo
EP
,
Valenza
G
, editors.
Complexity and nonlinearity in cardiovascular signals
.
Cham
:
Springer International Publishing
;
2017
. p.
345
74
.
39.
Koskima¨ki
H
,
Kinnunen
H
,
Ro¨nk¨a
S
,
Smarr
B
.
Following the heart: what does variation of resting heart rate tell about us as individuals and as a population
.
Adjunct proceedings of the 2019 ACM international joint conference on pervasive and ubiquitous computing and proceedings of the 2019 ACM international symposium on wearable computers
.
New York, 2019
:
UbiComp/ISWC ’19 Adjunct (Association for Computing Machinery
. p.
1178
81
.
40.
Azza
Y
,
Grueschow
M
,
Karlen
W
,
Seifritz
E
,
Kleim
B
.
How stress affects sleep and mental health: nocturnal heart rate increases during prolonged stress and interacts with childhood trauma exposure to predict anxiety
.
Sleep
.
2020
;
43
(
6
):
zsz310
.
41.
Woodward
SH
,
Jamison
AL
,
Gala
S
,
Lawlor
C
,
Villasenor
D
,
Tamayo
G
, et al
.
Heart rate during sleep in PTSD patients: moderation by contact with a service dog
.
Biol Psychol
.
2023
;
180
:
108586
.
42.
Siddi
S
,
Bailon
R
,
Giné-Vázquez
I
,
Matcham
F
,
Lamers
F
,
Kontaxis
S
.
PAB members
, et al
.
The usability of daytime and night-time heart rate dynamics as digital biomarkers of depression severity
.
Psychol Med
.
2023
;
53
(
8
):
3249
60
.
43.
Taillard
J
,
Lemoine
P
,
Boule
P
,
Drogue
M
,
Mouret
J
.
Sleep and heart rate circadian rhythm in depression: the necessity to separate
.
Chronobiol Int
.
1993
;
10
(
1
):
63
72
.
44.
Stampfer
HG
.
The relationship between psychiatric illness and the circadian pattern of heart rate
.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry
.
1998
;
32
(
2
):
187
98
.
45.
Hickie
IB
,
Naismith
SL
,
Robillard
R
,
Scott
EM
,
Hermens
DF
.
Manipulating the sleep-wake cycle and circadian rhythms to improve clinical management of major depression
.
BMC Med
.
2013
;
11
:
79
.
46.
Lutin
E
,
Schiweck
C
,
Cornelis
J
,
De Raedt
W
,
Reif
A
,
Vrieze
E
, et al
.
The cumulative effect of chronic stress and depressive symptoms affects heart rate in a working population
.
Front Psychiatry
.
2022
;
13
:
1022298
.
47.
Karimi
F
,
Amoozgar
Z
,
Reiazi
R
,
Hosseinzadeh
M
,
Rawassizadeh
R
.
Longitudinal analysis of heart rate and physical activity collected from smartwatches
.
CCF Trans Pervasive Comp Interact
.
2024
;
6
(
1
):
18
35
.
48.
Adlaf
EM
,
Gliksman
L
,
Demers
A
,
Newton-Taylor
B
.
The prevalence of elevated psychological distress among Canadian undergraduates: findings from the 1998 Canadian campus survey
.
J Am Coll Health
.
2001
;
50
(
2
):
67
72
.
49.
Becker
SP
,
Jarrett
MA
,
Luebbe
AM
,
Garner
AA
,
Burns
GL
,
Kofler
MJ
.
Sleep in a large, multi-university sample of college students: sleep problem prevalence, sex differences, and mental health correlates
.
Sleep Health
.
2018
;
4
(
2
):
174
81
.
50.
Adams
KL
,
Saunders
KE
,
Keown-Stoneman
CDG
,
Duffy
AC
.
Mental health trajectories in undergraduate students over the first year of university: a longitudinal cohort study
.
BMJ Open
.
2021
;
11
(
12
):
e047393
.
51.
Copeland
WE
,
McGinnis
E
,
Bai
Y
,
Adams
Z
,
Nardone
H
,
Devadanam
V
, et al
.
Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on college student mental health and wellness
.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
.
2021
;
60
(
1
):
134
41.e2
.
52.
Price
M
,
Hidalgo
JE
,
Bird
YM
,
Bloomfield
LSP
,
Buck
C
,
Cerutti
J
, et al
.
A large clinical trial to improve well-being during the transition to college using wearables: the Lived Experiences Measured Using Rings Study
.
Contemp Clin Trials
.
2023
;
133
:
107338
.
53.
Bloomfield
L
,
Fudolig
MI
,
Kim
J
,
Llorin
J
,
Lovato
JL
, et al
.
Predicting stress in first-year college students using sleep data from wearable devices
.
PLOS Digital Health
.
2024
;
3
(
4
). 10.1371/journal.pdig.0000473.
54.
Bloomfield
L
,
Fudolig
M
,
Dodds
P
,
Kim
J
,
Llorin
J
,
Lovato
J
, et al
.
Events and behaviors associated with symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder in first-year college students
.
PsyArXiv
.
2023
:
278ey
.
55.
Gray
MJ
,
Litz
BT
,
Hsu
JL
,
Lombardo
TW
.
Psychometric properties of the life events checklist
.
Assessment
.
2004
;
11
(
4
):
330
41
.
56.
Keogh
E
,
Chakrabarti
K
,
Pazzani
M
,
Mehrotra
S
.
Locally adaptive dimensionality reduction for indexing large time series databases
.
Proceedings of the 2001 ACM SIGMOD international Conference on Management of data, SIGMOD ’01
.
New York
:
Association for Computing Machinery
;
2001
. p.
151
62
.
57.
Lin
J
,
Keogh
E
,
Wei
L
,
Lonardi
S
.
Experiencing SAX: a novel symbolic representation of time series
.
Data Min Knowl Discov
.
2007
;
15
(
2
):
107
44
.
58.
Maier
M
,
Carlotto
H
,
Saperstein
S
,
Sanchez
F
,
Balogun
S
,
Merritt
S
.
Improving the accuracy and transparency of underwriting with artificial intelligence to transform the life-insurance industry
.
AI Mag
.
2020
;
41
(
3
):
78
93
.
59.
Cohen
S
,
Kamarck
T
,
Mermelstein
R
.
A global measure of perceived stress
.
J Health Soc Behav
.
1983
;
24
(
4
):
385
96
.
60.
Spitzer
RL
,
Kroenke
K
,
Williams
JBW
,
Löwe
B
.
A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7
.
Arch Intern Med
.
2006
;
166
(
10
):
1092
7
.
61.
MacQueen
J
.
Some methods for classification and analysis of multivariate observations
. In
Proceedings of the fifth berkeley symposium on mathematical statistics and probability
.
Statistics University of California Press
;
1967
.
Vol. 5.1
. p.
281
98
.
62.
Jolly
E
.
Pymer4: connecting R and Python for linear mixed modeling
.
J Open Source Softw
.
2018
;
3
(
31
):
862
.
63.
Hennig
C
.
Cluster-wise assessment of cluster stability
.
Comput Stat Data Anal
.
2007
;
52
(
1
):
258
71
.
64.
Oura Team
.
Sleeping heart rate: look for these 3 patterns
;
2023
. https://ouraring.com/blog/sleeping-heart-rate.
65.
Miano
S
,
Parisi
P
,
Villa
MP
.
The sleep phenotypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the role of arousal during sleep and implications for treatment
.
Med Hypotheses
.
2012
;
79
(
2
):
147
53
.
66.
Winsper
C
,
Tang
NKY
,
Marwaha
S
,
Lereya
ST
,
Gibbs
M
,
Thompson
A
, et al
.
The sleep phenotype of Borderline Personality Disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2017
;
73
:
48
67
.
67.
Miano
S
,
Amato
N
,
Foderaro
G
,
Pezzoli
V
,
Ramelli
GP
,
Toffolet
L
, et al
.
Sleep phenotypes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
.
Sleep Med
.
2019
;
60
:
123
31
.
68.
Hall
M
,
Vasko
R
,
Buysse
D
,
Ombao
H
,
Chen
Q
,
Cashmere
JD
, et al
.
Acute stress affects heart rate variability during sleep
.
Psychosom Med
.
2004
;
66
(
1
):
56
62
.
69.
Akerstedt
T
,
Kecklund
G
,
Axelsson
J
.
Impaired sleep after bedtime stress and worries
.
Biol Psychol
.
2007
;
76
(
3
):
170
3
.
70.
Drake
CL
,
Pillai
V
,
Roth
T
.
Stress and sleep reactivity: a prospective investigation of the stress-diathesis model of insomnia
.
Sleep
.
2014
;
37
(
8
):
1295
304
.
71.
Lindberg
E
,
Janson
C
,
Gislason
T
,
Björnsson
E
,
Hetta
J
,
Boman
G
.
Sleep disturbances in a young adult population: can gender differences Be explained by differences in psychological status
.
Sleep
.
1997
;
20
(
6
):
381
7
.
72.
Tsai
L-L
,
Li
S-P
.
Sleep patterns in college students: gender and grade differences
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2004
;
56
(
2
):
231
7
.
73.
Goel
N
,
Kim
H
,
Lao
RP
.
Gender differences in polysomnographic sleep in young healthy sleepers
.
Chronobiol Int
.
2005
;
22
(
5
):
905
15
.
74.
Mong
JA
,
Cusmano
DM
.
Sex differences in sleep: impact of biological sex and sex steroids
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
.
2016
;
371
(
1688
):
20150110
.
75.
Voderholzer
U
,
Al-Shajlawi
A
,
Weske
G
,
Feige
B
,
Riemann
D
.
Are there gender differences in objective and subjective sleep measures? A study of insomniacs and healthy controls
.
Depress Anxiety
.
2003
;
17
(
3
):
162
72
.
76.
Creswell
JD
,
Tumminia
MJ
,
Price
S
,
Sefidgar
Y
,
Cohen
S
,
Ren
Y
, et al
.
Nightly sleep duration predicts grade point average in the first year of college
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
.
2023
;
120
:e2209123120.
77.
Liyanage
S
,
Saqib
K
,
Khan
AF
,
Thobani
TR
,
Tang
W-C
,
Chiarot
CB
, et al
.
Prevalence of anxiety in university students during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2021
;
19
(
1
):
62
.
78.
Senaratna
CV
,
Perret
JL
,
Lodge
CJ
,
Lowe
AJ
,
Campbell
BE
,
Matheson
MC
, et al
.
Prevalence of obstructive sleep apnea in the general population: a systematic review
.
Sleep Med Rev
.
2017
;
34
:
70
81
.
79.
Gebreab
SZ
,
Vandeleur
CL
,
Rudaz
D
,
Strippoli
M-PF
,
Gholam-Rezaee
M
,
Castelao
E
, et al
.
Psychosocial stress over the lifespan, psychological factors, and cardiometabolic risk in the community
.
Psychosom Med
.
2018
;
80
(
7
):
628
39
.