Introduction: Neighborhood socioeconomic status (NSES) has been linked with overall health, and this study will evaluate whether NSES is cross-sectionally associated with cognition in non-Hispanic whites (NHWs) and Mexican Americans (MAs) from the Health and Aging Brain: Health Disparities Study (HABS-HD). Methods: The HABS-HD is a longitudinal study conducted at the University of North Texas Health Science Center. The final sample analyzed (n = 1,312) were 50 years or older, with unimpaired cognition, and underwent an interview, neuropsychological examination, imaging, and blood draw. NSES was measured using the national area deprivation index (ADI) percentile ranking, which considered socioeconomic variables. Executive function and processing speed were assessed by the trail making tests (A and B) and the digit-symbol substitution test, respectively. Linear regression was used to assess the association of ADI and cognitive measures. Results: MAs were younger, more likely to be female, less educated, had higher ADI scores, performed worse on trails B (all p < 0.05), and had lower prevalence of APOE4 + when compared to NHWs (p < 0.0001). A higher percentage of MAs lived in the most deprived neighborhoods than NHWs. For NHWs, ADI did not predict trails B or DSS scores, after adjusting for demographic variables and APOE4. For MAs, ADI predicted trails A, trails B, and DSS after adjusting for demographic covariates and APOE4 status. Conclusion: Our study revealed that living in an area of higher deprivation was associated with lower cognitive function in MAs but not in NHWs, which is important to consider in future interventions to slow cognitive decline.

Dementia is increasing in frequency worldwide. According to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), in the USA, the prevalence in adults over the age of 65 will rise from 5.6 million in 2014 to 14 million in 2060, with the largest increases in Latino/Hispanic (hereafter Hispanic) and African Americans [1]. Dementia is a group of disorders that influence the daily lives of individuals through changes in memory, behavior, language, and other cognitive abilities [2], with a significant economic burden for individuals, families, and society. Research suggests that many individual factors (e.g. age, education, diabetes, hypertension, obesity, among others) are associated with cognitive decline in elders [3, 4]. Recently, investigators have focused on social determinants of health that have a broader impact on individual’s health and specifically those that are a risk factor for cognitive decline [5, 6]. A growing body of research has shown that living in a deprived neighborhood affects physical health, increases cardiovascular morbidity and mortality rates, and has links with lower cognitive function regardless of individual level characteristics [5‒8]. Neighborhood deprivation is a key contributor of health inequalities seen in Hispanic and other minorities [9]. Multiple mechanisms have been proposed to explain the neighborhood effects on health. Neighborhood socioeconomic status (NSES) has been linked to psychosocial stress [10], poor health behaviors, low access to health care, dangerous environments [11], green space availability, and air pollution [12]. The mechanisms responsible for the link between NSES and cognition are not well understood. Living in a disadvantaged neighborhood is associated with a higher prevalence of cardiovascular risk factors [13]; these factors have a well-established association with brain health and cognition [14].

The area deprivation index (ADI), a validated measure of state and national neighborhood disadvantage in the USA, has been associated with cognitive impairment [15] and brain structural changes in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) signature regions [16]. Furthermore, in 2020, Powell et al. analyzed 447 autopsy samples and found that living in disadvantaged neighborhoods at the time of death was associated with increased neurofibrillary tangles and amyloid plaques, key pathological markers of AD [17].

The ADI has been associated with cognitive outcomes, particularly with executive functioning measured by the trail making test, part B [18, 19]. Socioeconomic status has been associated with lower processing speed [20]. Beck et al. [21] in 2018 used multiple mediation analyses to examine the direct and indirect effects of childhood socioeconomic status in multiple cognitive domains in midlife. They showed that men from lower child socioeconomic status performed worse during midlife in many cognitive measures including processing speed. Despite the rapid growth of the US Mexican American (MA) population, studies examining neighborhood disadvantaged and cognitive performance have primarily focused on non-Hispanic whites (NHWs), with few studies focusing on MAs [22, 23]. Research has shown that Hispanics living in the most disadvantaged areas have worse cognitive performance than other Hispanics living in the least deprived neighborhoods, and NHWs living in similar disadvantaged conditions [24]. This gap must be addressed to develop prevention, care, and treatment interventions that focus on this underrepresented population.

The current study examines whether NSES is cross-sectionally associated with performance on measures of executive functioning and processing speed among NHWs, and MAs from the Health and Aging Brain: Health Disparities Study (HABS-HD). We hypothesized that living in the most disadvantaged neighborhoods would be associated with poorer cognitive performance. Based on prior research [25], we also hypothesized that associations would be stronger in MAs than in NHWs.

Sample

Data from the HABS-HD, a community-based longitudinal study of cognitive aging, conducted by the Institute for Translational Research at the University of North Texas Health Science Center was analyzed. Participants are recruited from the Dallas/Fort Worth metropolitan area in Texas. The HABS-HD study inclusion criteria include age 50 and older, self-identified as MAs or NHWs, willingness and capable of undergoing blood draws and neuroimaging procedures, and fluency in Spanish or English. Exclusion criteria include Type 1 diabetes, current inflammatory conditions like lupus, current diagnoses of cancer, mental illness (except depression), a traumatic brain injury with loss of consciousness in the last year, and other severe medical conditions like end stage renal failure, which preclude participation. HABS-HD participants undergo a clinical interview, neuropsychological testing, functional examination, MRI of the head, amyloid PET scan, and blood draw for clinical and biomarkers analysis. All study components are conducted in English or Spanish based on participant preference. Validated Spanish versions of cognitive tests were used, and native Spanish speakers administered the test. A complete description of the HABS-HD study has been published elsewhere [26].

The HABS-HD study was approved by the North Texas Regional Institutional Review Board. Each participant, or legal representative for those with cognitive impairment, signs a written informed consent. All HABS-HD data is available to the scientific community through the UNTHSC Institute for Translational Research (ITR) website [27]. From March 2017 to June 2022, 2,380 subjects were enrolled in the study baseline visit. To analyze the relationship of ADI and cognition in a cognitively unimpaired cohort, 380 participants with a diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and 155 participants with an AD diagnosis were excluded. Of the remaining 1,845 participants, 316 were excluded due to insufficient demographic information (zip code) to obtain the ADI. Additionally, 217 participants who did not have complete neuropsychological tests data were excluded. The final sample of 1,312 met the following criteria: (1) provided zip code to obtain ADI, (2) underwent all tests of interest, (3) did not meet criteria for MCI or AD, and (4) had complete data on covariates including age, sex, education, ethnicity, and APOE4 status. Of the final sample, 652 were NHWs, 660 were MAs, 63.7% were female, and the mean age was 65.7 years old. Participants excluded from the analysis did not significantly differ from the final sample participants in age nor ADI scores. Participants excluded from the study had less education, performed worse in all cognitive measures, were less likely to be female, and had a higher percentage of APOE4 positivity (online suppl. Table 1; for all online suppl. material, see https://doi.org/10.1159/000539035)

Cognitive Diagnosis

Cognitive diagnoses are assigned using an algorithm (decision tree) that is verified at consensus review. Unimpaired Cognition (UC) = no cognitive complaints, clinical dementia rating (CDR) sum of boxes score of 0 and cognitive test scores broadly within normal limits (i.e. performance no more than 1.5 standard deviations below the mean of the normative range on any test). For the current study, only those meeting the criteria for unimpaired cognition were included.

Area Deprivation Index

The Area Deprivation Index is based on 17 census variables including education, employment, income, occupation, and housing, to create a composite measure of neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage [28]. The ADI allows for comparisons at the census-block group level, and is publicly available at https://www.neigborhoodatlas.medicine.wisc.edu/. For analysis, we used the national ADI percentile ranking as a continuous variable, with scores ranging from 1 to 100. A score/ranking of 1 indicates the lowest level of neighborhood disadvantage (least deprived), and a ranking/score of 100 indicates the highest level of neighborhood disadvantage (most deprived). ADI quartiles were used to assess the differences in baseline characteristics and cognitive scores, comparing the least deprived neighborhoods (quartile 1) to the most deprived neighborhood (quartile 4) within groups, and the differences in quartile 4 between groups.

Cognitive Function

The trail making test (TMT) is widely used to assess executive function and reflects a variety of mental abilities such as attention, mental flexibility, visual scanning, psychomotor speed, and abstraction [29]. The TMT consists of parts A and B. In part A, the participants connect a series of 25 numbers in circles in order, while in part B, circles contain numbers and letters that participants need to connect in numerical and alphabetical order, alternating between numbers and letters. The total time for completion is the variable of interest. Trail A primarily tests motor speed, and trail B is a test of higher-level cognitive skills [30]. The digit-symbol substitution test (DSST) assesses processing speed, attention, and visuoperceptual functions [31]. It has been shown that executive functioning also contributes to the performance in this test [32].

Covariates

Age, sex, total years of education, and APOE4 status were entered as covariates. Participants self-reported age, sex, ethnicity, and total years of education during the interview.

Statistical Analysis

To facilitate effect size comparisons between variables, we used neuropsychological tests z-scores obtained from normative data for Texas MA Adults [33]. The signs of z-scores for trails A and B, where a higher score indicates a worse outcome, were inverted before statistical comparisons with ADI were performed. After this transformation, a higher z-score indicated a better performance for all tests. Following the transformation of the scores, the cross-sectional association between ADI and the cognitive measures was examined.

Baseline differences between MAs and NHWs were analyzed using t tests for continuous variables, and χ2 tests for categorical variables. Previous research showed that individuals in the top 20% most deprived neighborhoods had the most impact on health outcomes [28]. Therefore, the differences of baseline characteristics between participants living in the least deprived neighborhoods (ADI quartile 1) and those living in the most deprived neighborhoods (ADI quartile 4) within ethnic groups were compared. The differences between ethnic groups among participants in ADI quartile 4, were also analyzed using t-tests and chi-squared tests.

Moderation analysis was conducted using PROCESS macro (Hayes 2013) to assess the effect of the interaction of ADI and ethnicity on cognitive scores. Then, after checking for linearity (Fig. 1-3), a hierarchical linear regression, stratified by ethnicity, was performed to assess the association between ADI as a continuous variable, and each cognitive test score as a separate outcome. Model 1 was adjusted for age and APOE4, Model 2 was adjusted for age, APOE4, and sex, and Model 3 was adjusted for age, APOE4, sex, and education.

Fig. 1.

Scatter plot of trails A scores by ADI rank. Y-axis corresponds to trails A z-scores and X-axis corresponds to ADI national rank scores.

Fig. 1.

Scatter plot of trails A scores by ADI rank. Y-axis corresponds to trails A z-scores and X-axis corresponds to ADI national rank scores.

Close modal
Fig. 2.

Scatter plot of trails B scores by ADI rank. Y-axis corresponds to trails B z-scores and X-axis corresponds to ADI national rank scores.

Fig. 2.

Scatter plot of trails B scores by ADI rank. Y-axis corresponds to trails B z-scores and X-axis corresponds to ADI national rank scores.

Close modal
Fig. 3.

Scatter plot of DSS scores by ADI rank. Y-axis corresponds to trails A z-scores and X-axis corresponds to ADI national rank scores.

Fig. 3.

Scatter plot of DSS scores by ADI rank. Y-axis corresponds to trails A z-scores and X-axis corresponds to ADI national rank scores.

Close modal

Baseline Characteristics

The total analyzed sample of 1,312 participants (n = 652 – NHW and n = 660 – MA) had a mean age of 65.7 ± 8.4 years, and 64% were female. Baseline characteristics are presented in Table 1. MAs were significantly younger, less educated, had higher ADI scores, and had a higher proportion of female participants when compared to NHWs. MA participants performed worse in trail B. A higher percentage of NHWs were APOE4 carriers.

Table 1.

Demographics by ethnicity

Total sample (N = 1,312), mean (SD)NHW (N = 652), mean (SD)MA (N = 660), mean (SD)t (95% CI)p value
Age 65.73 (8.48) 68.65 (8.33) 62.85 (7.6) −13.17 (−6.66 to −4.93) <0.001 
Education 12.76 (4.61) 15.6 (2.58) 9.95 (4.46) −28.04 (−6.04 to −5.25) <0.001 
ADI national 57.44 (26.94) 42.94 (22.88) 71.76 (22.65) 22.92 (26.35–31.28) <0.001 
Trails A 0.2 (0.77) 0.18 (0.71) 0.22 (0.82) 0.94 (−0.04–0.12) 0.34 
Trails B −0.06 (1.12) 0.11 (0.88) −0.24 (1.28) −5.76 (−0.46 to −0.23) <0.001 
DSS 0.22 (0.85) 0.19 (0.85) 0.26 (0.86) 1.48 (−0.02–0.16) 0.13 
Total sample (N = 1,312), mean (SD)NHW (N = 652), mean (SD)MA (N = 660), mean (SD)t (95% CI)p value
Age 65.73 (8.48) 68.65 (8.33) 62.85 (7.6) −13.17 (−6.66 to −4.93) <0.001 
Education 12.76 (4.61) 15.6 (2.58) 9.95 (4.46) −28.04 (−6.04 to −5.25) <0.001 
ADI national 57.44 (26.94) 42.94 (22.88) 71.76 (22.65) 22.92 (26.35–31.28) <0.001 
Trails A 0.2 (0.77) 0.18 (0.71) 0.22 (0.82) 0.94 (−0.04–0.12) 0.34 
Trails B −0.06 (1.12) 0.11 (0.88) −0.24 (1.28) −5.76 (−0.46 to −0.23) <0.001 
DSS 0.22 (0.85) 0.19 (0.85) 0.26 (0.86) 1.48 (−0.02–0.16) 0.13 
Total sample, N (%)NHW, N (%)MA, N (%)χ2 (95% CI)p value
Gender (female) 836 (63.7) 381 (58.4) 455 (68.9) 15.62 (5.29–15.62) 0.001 
Hypertension (yes) 797 (60.7) 374 (57.4) 423 (64.1) 6.17 (1.41–11.93) 0.01 
Diabetes (yes) 300 (22.9) 75 (11.5) 225 (34.1) 94.9 (18.18–26.91) <0.001 
Dyslipidemia (yes) 860 (64.8) 415 (63.7) 435 (65.9) 0.69 (−2.96–7.34) 0.4 
Obesity (yes) 588 (44.8) 251 (38.5) 337 (51.1) 21.03 (7.22–17.87) <0.001 
APOE4 (yes) (1,196) 274 (20.9) 169 (25.9) 105 (15.9) 18.05 (5.4–14.54) <0.001 
Total sample, N (%)NHW, N (%)MA, N (%)χ2 (95% CI)p value
Gender (female) 836 (63.7) 381 (58.4) 455 (68.9) 15.62 (5.29–15.62) 0.001 
Hypertension (yes) 797 (60.7) 374 (57.4) 423 (64.1) 6.17 (1.41–11.93) 0.01 
Diabetes (yes) 300 (22.9) 75 (11.5) 225 (34.1) 94.9 (18.18–26.91) <0.001 
Dyslipidemia (yes) 860 (64.8) 415 (63.7) 435 (65.9) 0.69 (−2.96–7.34) 0.4 
Obesity (yes) 588 (44.8) 251 (38.5) 337 (51.1) 21.03 (7.22–17.87) <0.001 
APOE4 (yes) (1,196) 274 (20.9) 169 (25.9) 105 (15.9) 18.05 (5.4–14.54) <0.001 

DSS, digit symbol substitution; SD, standard deviation.

t test for continuous variables and χ2 for categorical variables were used for comparison between ethnicities.

Cognitive scores are z-scores.

Significance was set at p ≤ 0.05.

Table 2 shows the differences between ADI quartiles 1 and 4 within groups. NHWs living in the most disadvantaged neighborhoods (ADI quartile 4) had fewer years of education, and scored worse in trails A and trails B, when compared with those living in the least deprived neighborhoods (ADI quartile 1). For MAs, those living in ADI quartile 4 were older, less educated, and had lower scores in trails A, trails B, and DSS, than their counterparts in ADI quartile 1.

Table 2.

Demographics by ADI quartiles by ethnicity

NHW (652)MA (660)
quartile 1 (159)quartile 2 (256)quartile 3 (158)quartile 4 (79)quartile 1 (34)quartile 2 (89)quartile 3 (148)quartile 4 (389)
Age 68.97 (8.13) 69.18 (8.28) 67.89 (8.43) 67.76 (8.69) 61.79 (7.66) 63.93 (7.76) 62.39 (7.15) 62.87 (7.71) 
Education in years 16.4 (2.14) 15.79 (2.53) 15.18 (2.6) 14.15 (2.82)* 14.68 (3.11) 12.6 (3.99) 11.01 (4.6)3 8.53 (3.91)* 
Trails A 0.25 (0.64) 0.15 (0.72) 0.2 (0.73) 0.06 (0.78)* 0.56 (0.63) 0.36 (0.75) 0.19 (0.76) 0.17 (0.86)* 
Trails B 0.21 (0.8) 0.11 (0.8) 0.15 (0.83) −0.16 (1.27)* 0.34 (0.83) 0.32 (0.87) −0.17 (1.14) −0.45 (1.39)* 
DSS 0.29 (0.8) 0.18 (0.91) 0.23 (0.78) −0.05 (0.8)* 0.61 (0.72) 0.38 (0.83) 0.21 (0.89) 0.22 (0.86)* 
Gender (female) 87 (54.7) 147 (57.4) 94 (59.5) 53 (67.1) 24 (70.6) 55 (61.8) 98 (66.2) 278 (71.5) 
APOE4 (yes) (1,196) 31 (19.5) 69 (27) 45 (28.5) 24 (30.4) 10 (29.4) 11 (12.4) 23 (15.5) 61 (15.7) 
NHW (652)MA (660)
quartile 1 (159)quartile 2 (256)quartile 3 (158)quartile 4 (79)quartile 1 (34)quartile 2 (89)quartile 3 (148)quartile 4 (389)
Age 68.97 (8.13) 69.18 (8.28) 67.89 (8.43) 67.76 (8.69) 61.79 (7.66) 63.93 (7.76) 62.39 (7.15) 62.87 (7.71) 
Education in years 16.4 (2.14) 15.79 (2.53) 15.18 (2.6) 14.15 (2.82)* 14.68 (3.11) 12.6 (3.99) 11.01 (4.6)3 8.53 (3.91)* 
Trails A 0.25 (0.64) 0.15 (0.72) 0.2 (0.73) 0.06 (0.78)* 0.56 (0.63) 0.36 (0.75) 0.19 (0.76) 0.17 (0.86)* 
Trails B 0.21 (0.8) 0.11 (0.8) 0.15 (0.83) −0.16 (1.27)* 0.34 (0.83) 0.32 (0.87) −0.17 (1.14) −0.45 (1.39)* 
DSS 0.29 (0.8) 0.18 (0.91) 0.23 (0.78) −0.05 (0.8)* 0.61 (0.72) 0.38 (0.83) 0.21 (0.89) 0.22 (0.86)* 
Gender (female) 87 (54.7) 147 (57.4) 94 (59.5) 53 (67.1) 24 (70.6) 55 (61.8) 98 (66.2) 278 (71.5) 
APOE4 (yes) (1,196) 31 (19.5) 69 (27) 45 (28.5) 24 (30.4) 10 (29.4) 11 (12.4) 23 (15.5) 61 (15.7) 

Continuous variables reported as mean and standard deviation.

Categorical variables reported as number and percentage.

DSS, digit symbol substitution.

Cognitive scores are z-scores.

Significant difference between quartile 1 and quartile 4 within ethnic groups.

*p ≤ 0.05.

The ethnic group differences between NHWs and MAs living in the most deprived neighborhoods (Table 3) showed that a higher percentage of MAs live in the most disadvantaged neighborhoods (χ2 = 312.93, 95% CI = 42.12–51.14, p = <0.01). MAs in ADI quartile 4 were younger, less educated, and had lower scores in DSS, when compared with NHWs in ADI quartile 4.

Table 3.

Quartile 4 demographics by ethnicity

NHW (79)MA (660)
Age 67.76 (8.69) 62.87 (7.71)* 
Education in years 14.15 (2.82) 8.53 (3.91)* 
Trails A 0.06 (0.78) 0.17 (0.86) 
Trails B −0.16 (1.27) −0.45 (1.39) 
DSS −0.05 (0.8) 0.22 (0.86)* 
Gender (female) 53 (67.1) 278 (71.5) 
APOE4 (yes) (1,196) 24 (30.4) 61 (15.7)* 
NHW (79)MA (660)
Age 67.76 (8.69) 62.87 (7.71)* 
Education in years 14.15 (2.82) 8.53 (3.91)* 
Trails A 0.06 (0.78) 0.17 (0.86) 
Trails B −0.16 (1.27) −0.45 (1.39) 
DSS −0.05 (0.8) 0.22 (0.86)* 
Gender (female) 53 (67.1) 278 (71.5) 
APOE4 (yes) (1,196) 24 (30.4) 61 (15.7)* 

Continuous variables reported as mean and standard deviation.

Categorical variables reported as number and percentage.

Significant difference in baseline characteristics between ethnic groups.

DSS, digit symbol substitution.

Cognitive scores are z-scores.

*p ≤ 0.05.

Area Deprivation Index and Cognitive Function

Moderation analysis (online suppl. Table 2) showed that the interaction between ADI and ethnicity was not significant for trail A and DSS (p > 0.05). However, the interaction between ADI and ethnicity was significant for trails B (b = −0.01, SE = 0.002, t = −3.86, p < 0.01). The simple slope of ADI on trail B was significant for Hispanic race (b = −0.013, SE = −0.001, t = −7.37, p < 0.01) but not for NHW race (b = 0.003, SE = 0.001, t = −1.92, p = 0.054).

Table 4 presents results from three linear regression models for the cross-sectional association between ADI and transformed cognitive scores. After adjusting for age, APOE4 status, sex, and education, ADI did not predict cognitive scores in NHWs. In MAs, the relationship between ADI and trails A (B = −0.004, p < 0.015), trails B (B = −0.005, p = 0.017), and DSS (B = −0.003, p = 0.02) remained significant after adjusting for covariates.

Table 4.

ADI as predictor of cognitive scores by ethnicity

NHWMA
Btp valueBtp value
Model 1 
 Trails A −0.002 −1.77 0.07 −0.005 −3.38 <0.001 
 Trails B −0.004 −2.58 0.01 −0.014 −6.52 <0.001 
 DSS −0.004 −2.55 0.01 −0.004 −2.41 0.01 
Model 2 
 Trails A −0.002 −1.76 0.07 −0.005 −3.43 <0.001 
 Trails B −0.004 −2.55 0.01 −0.014 −6.58 <0.001 
 DSS −0.004 −2.89 <0.01 −0.004 −2.58 0.01 
Model 3 
 Trails A −0.001 −1.11 0.26 −0.004 −2.83 <0.01 
 Trails B −0.003 −1.63 0.10 −0.005 −2.39 0.01 
 DSS −0.003 −1.95 0.051 −0.004 −2.25 0.02 
NHWMA
Btp valueBtp value
Model 1 
 Trails A −0.002 −1.77 0.07 −0.005 −3.38 <0.001 
 Trails B −0.004 −2.58 0.01 −0.014 −6.52 <0.001 
 DSS −0.004 −2.55 0.01 −0.004 −2.41 0.01 
Model 2 
 Trails A −0.002 −1.76 0.07 −0.005 −3.43 <0.001 
 Trails B −0.004 −2.55 0.01 −0.014 −6.58 <0.001 
 DSS −0.004 −2.89 <0.01 −0.004 −2.58 0.01 
Model 3 
 Trails A −0.001 −1.11 0.26 −0.004 −2.83 <0.01 
 Trails B −0.003 −1.63 0.10 −0.005 −2.39 0.01 
 DSS −0.003 −1.95 0.051 −0.004 −2.25 0.02 

Model 1: adjusted for age and APOE4. Model 2: adjusted for age, APOE4, and sex. Model 3: adjusted for age, APOE4, sex, and education.

B, unstandardized B; t, t value; p, p value; DSS, digit symbol substitution.

Cognitive scores are z-scores.

Significance was set at p ≤ 0.05.

This cross-sectional data analysis demonstrated an association between neighborhood disadvantage and poorer cognition focused on aspects of executive function and processing speed in this bi-ethnic cohort. Both NHWs and MAs living in the most disadvantaged neighborhoods performed worse on trails A, trails B, and the digit symbol substitution. These findings are consistent with previous work examining the association of neighborhood disadvantage and cognition [19, 34]. However, when NHWs were compared to MAs living within the same level of neighborhood deprivation (ADI quartile 4), performance on DSS was the only measure that showed a significant difference between the ethnic groups.

For the two groups, the relationship between cognition and ADI was differentially impacted by demographic factors and the presence of APOE4. After adjusting for covariates (demographic variables and APOE4 status), ADI was associated with lower scores in executive function and processing speed only in MAs. These findings did not corroborate previous research in NHW populations. In a cross-sectional analysis of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, Lang et al. [8] concluded that higher neighborhood socioeconomic deprivation was associated with lower cognitive functioning. Similar results were reported by Zuelsdorff et al. [28] in a sample composed of 88.6% NHWs. Researchers found that subjects living in the 20% most disadvantaged neighborhoods performed lower in many cognitive domains, including executive function. The small proportion of NHW participants within the most disadvantaged neighborhood (ADI quartile 4), may have prevented finding a potential relationship between ADI and cognition in this ethnic group. Another consideration is the low power due to small sample size, which may explain the difference in findings between MAs and NHWs. A new study reported that low-income white people tend to live in better neighborhoods than middle-class Hispanic [35], which may explain the lower percentage of NHWs living in ADI quartile 4. Even at the same ADI level (quartile 4), MAs had significantly fewer years of education, and higher prevalence of CVRF like diabetes, both of which are related to cognition. These differences in individual factors may explain some of the differences found between ethnic groups. Phelan and Link suggested that it is not only socioeconomic status that is a cause of health disparities, but that ethnicity also plays a role in health and mortality [36]. Racial discrimination produces stress and could contribute to deficits in cognition. Researchers have found evidence showing minorities perceived stress as a mediator between SES and lower cognitive scores in multiple cognitive domains [37]. Negative effects in executive function have been reported in individuals who experienced or observed subtle discrimination which contributes to academic, employment, and health inequalities [38].

Many potential pathways for neighborhood deprivation to impact cognition have been postulated. The lack of recreation centers, parks, and green spaces could promote lower physical activity [39]. Lower social activity and support, and higher levels of anxiety and depression as a result of the absence of cultural centers, community centers, and the presence of social stressors (e.g. violence, substance abuse) may increase the risk of cognitive impairment [40]. Cardiovascular risk is influenced by the socioeconomic environment and is associated with an increased risk of dementia [41]. In an elderly cohort, socioeconomic factors such as income, education, and a measure of neighborhood disadvantage, were associated with asymptomatic cardiovascular disease, independent of age, gender, and sex [42]. A novel mechanistic pathway, epigenetic age, has been studied. Smith et al. [43] in 2017 concluded that neighborhood status may influence methylation and gene expression, even after controlling for individual socioeconomic factors. The weathering hypothesis as described by Simmons and colleagues, whereby chronic exposure to social and economic disadvantage accelerates adverse health outcomes through DNA methylation changes, may help in the understanding of the results from this study [44]. Currently, the HABS-HD study is capturing genomic data that will allow us to assess the role of epigenetic factors in the association of ADI and cognition. More research is needed to elucidate the pathophysiological pathways through which neighborhood disadvantage affects cognition across the individual life span.

Limitations of this study include the cross-sectional design; thus, causal relationships must be interpreted with caution. Residential history and changes in the living environment through life were not considered, so the association of early-life socioeconomic disadvantage with poor cognitive health was not accounted for. As the HABS-HD study is longitudinal, and is adding a younger cohort, future research will investigate the impact of these variables over time. Other possible confounding factors like income, smoking status, physical activity, diet, kidney disease, among others, need to be addressed in future research. As explained above, the percentage of NHW participants living in the most deprived neighborhoods is small when compared to MAs, which may have affected comparisons. We excluded participants with MCI and AD diagnoses which may have contributed to bias in the associations. Finally, we did not do a sensitivity analysis imputing covariate missing data, and this may affect the results.

This study in a bi-ethnic sample provides evidence of the association of neighborhood disadvantage and cognitive performance. Residing in a neighborhood with higher deprivation was associated with lower cognitive scores in MAs but not in NHWs. This is important as processes that affect neighborhood status and cognition may differ for MAs when compared to other ethnic groups. Cognitive risk is not only a result of behavior or personal traits, but also where individuals live. Findings highlighted the need for preventive strategies and interventions targeting neighborhood conditions and modifiable risk factors of at-risk populations like MAs. Future research that includes other cognitive domains and individual socio-economic factors is warranted to study the longitudinal relationship of NSES and the increased risk for cognitive decline.

The research team thanks the local Fort Worth community and participants of the HABS-HD study.

This study protocol was reviewed and approved by the North Texas Regional Institution Review Board approval number 2016-128. Each participant signed a written informed consent.

L.A.J. has a financial interest in CX Precision Medicine, Inc. S.E.O. has multiple patents on precision medicine for neurodegenerative diseases and is the scientific founder of CX Precision Medicine, Inc. RV, AB, RM, and JH declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Numbers R01AG054073, R01AG058533, P41EB015922, U19AG078109, and R35AG071916. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

R.V. and J.H. conceived the idea. R.V., A.B., and R.M. equally contributed to drafting the manuscript. J.R.H., L.A.J., and S.E.O. provided critical review, editing, and input. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

All HABS-HD data are available to the scientific community through the UNTHSC Institute for Translational Research (ITR) website: https://apps.unthsc.edu/itr/. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

1.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
.
Reducing risk of Alzheimer’s disease
. (cited 2023 Aug 24). Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/aging/publications/features/reducing-risk-of-alzheimers-disease/index.htm
2.
Christopher
G
.
What is dementia
.
Dement
.
2023
:
1
13
.
3.
Baumgart
M
,
Snyder
HM
,
Carrillo
MC
,
Fazio
S
,
Kim
H
,
Johns
H
.
Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population‐based perspective
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2015
;
11
(
6
):
718
26
.
4.
Yang
HW
,
Bae
JB
,
Oh
DJ
,
Moon
DG
,
Lim
E
,
Shin
J
, et al
.
Exploration of cognitive outcomes and risk factors for cognitive decline shared by couples
.
JAMA Netw Open
.
2021
;
4
(
12
):
e2139765
.
5.
Majoka
MA
,
Schimming
C
.
Effect of social determinants of health on Cognition and risk of Alzheimer disease and related dementias
.
Clin Ther
.
2021
;
43
(
6
):
922
9
.
6.
Hilal
S
,
Brayne
C
.
Epidemiologic trends, Social Determinants, and Brain Health: the role of life course inequalities
.
Stroke
.
2022
;
53
(
2
):
437
43
.
7.
Diez Roux
AV
.
Investigating neighborhood and area effects on health
.
Am J Public Health
.
2001
;
91
(
11
):
1783
9
.
8.
Lang
IA
,
Llewellyn
DJ
,
Langa
KM
,
Wallace
RB
,
Huppert
FA
,
Melzer
D
.
Neighborhood deprivation, individual socioeconomic status, and cognitive function in older people: analyses from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2008
;
56
(
2
):
191
8
.
9.
Gallo
LC
,
Savin
KL
,
Jankowska
MM
,
Roesch
SC
,
Sallis
JF
,
Sotres-Alvarez
D
, et al
.
Neighborhood environment and metabolic risk in hispanics/latinos from the hispanic community health study/study of latinos
.
Am J Prev Med
.
2022
;
63
(
2
):
195
203
.
10.
Kwarteng
JL
,
Schulz
AJ
,
Mentz
GB
,
Israel
BA
,
Perkins
DW
.
Independent effects of neighborhood poverty and psychosocial stress on obesity over time
.
J Urban Health
.
2017
;
94
(
6
):
791
802
.
11.
Senn
TE
,
Walsh
JL
,
Carey
MP
.
The mediating roles of perceived stress and health behaviors in the relation between objective, subjective, and neighborhood socioeconomic status and perceived health
.
Ann Behav Med
.
2014
;
48
(
2
):
215
24
.
12.
Chaparro
MP
,
Benzeval
M
,
Richardson
E
,
Mitchell
R
.
Neighborhood deprivation and biomarkers of health in Britain: the mediating role of the Physical Environment
.
BMC Public Health
.
2018
;
18
(
1
):
801
.
13.
Jimenez
MP
,
Wellenius
GA
,
Subramanian
SV
,
Buka
S
,
Eaton
C
,
Gilman
SE
, et al
.
Longitudinal Associations of neighborhood socioeconomic status with cardiovascular risk factors: a 46-year follow-up study
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2019
;
241
:
112574
.
14.
Stewart
RAH
,
Held
C
,
Krug-Gourley
S
,
Waterworth
D
,
Stebbins
A
,
Chiswell
K
, et al
.
Cardiovascular and lifestyle risk factors and cognitive function in patients with stable coronary heart disease
.
JAMA
.
2019
;
8
(
7
):
e010641
.
15.
Ysea‐Hill
O
,
Gomez
C
,
Shah
A
,
Ruiz
SJ
,
Ruiz
JG
.
Cross‐Sectional Association between the area deprivation index (ADI) and cognitive impairment in community‐dwelling older veterans
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2021
;
17
(
S7
):
e052562
n/a2021-12
.
16.
Vassilaki
M
,
Petersen
RC
,
Vemuri
P
.
Area deprivation index as a surrogate of resilience in aging and dementia
.
Front Psychol
.
2022
;
13
:
930415
.
17.
Powell
WR
,
Buckingham
WR
,
Larson
JL
,
Vilen
L
,
Yu
M
,
Salamat
S
, et al
.
Association of neighborhood-level disadvantage with Alzheimer disease neuropathology
.
JAMA Netw Open
.
2020
;
3
(
6
):
e207559
.
18.
Hunt
JFV
,
Vogt
NM
,
Jonaitis
EM
,
Buckingham
WR
,
Koscik
RL
,
Zuelsdorff
M
, et al
.
Neighborhood disadvantage is associated with accelerated cortical thinning and cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired adults
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2020
;
16
(
S10
):
e043170
.
19.
Hunt
JFV
,
Vogt
NM
,
Jonaitis
EM
,
Buckingham
WR
,
Koscik
RL
,
Zuelsdorff
M
, et al
.
Association of neighborhood context, cognitive decline, and cortical change in an unimpaired cohort
.
Neurology
.
2021
;
96
(
20
):
e2500
12
.
20.
Steptoe
A
,
Zaninotto
P
.
Lower socioeconomic status and the acceleration of aging: an outcome-wide analysis
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2020
;
117
(
26
):
14911
7
.
21.
Beck
A
,
Franz
CE
,
Xian
H
,
Vuoksimaa
E
,
Tu
X
,
Reynolds
CA
, et al
.
Mediators of the effect of childhood socioeconomic status on late midlife cognitive abilities: a Four Decade longitudinal study
.
Innov Aging
.
2018
;
2
(
1
):
igy003
.
22.
Zeki Al Hazzouri
A
,
Haan
MN
,
Kalbfleisch
JD
,
Galea
S
,
Lisabeth
LD
,
Aiello
AE
.
Life-course socioeconomic position and incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment without dementia in older Mexican Americans: results from the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2011
;
173
(
10
):
1148
58
.
23.
Rote
S
,
Angel
JL
.
Gender-based pathways to cognitive aging in the Mexican-origin population in the United States: the significance of work and family
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
2021
;
76
(
4
):
e165
75
.
24.
Mehdipanah
R
,
Briceño
EM
,
Heeringa
SG
,
Gonzales
XF
,
Levine
DA
,
Langa
KM
, et al
.
Neighborhood SES and cognitive function among Hispanic/Latinx residents: why where you live matters
.
Am J Prev Med
.
2022
;
63
(
4
):
574
81
.
25.
Wong
CG
,
Miller
JB
,
Zhang
F
,
Rissman
RA
,
Raman
R
,
Hall
JR
, et al
.
Evaluation of neighborhood-level disadvantage and cognition in Mexican American and non-Hispanic White adults 50 years and older in the US
.
JAMA Netw Open
.
2023
;
6
(
8
):
e2325325
.
26.
O’Bryant
SE
,
Johnson
LA
,
Barber
RC
,
Braskie
MN
,
Christian
B
,
Hall
JR
, et al
.
The health and aging brain among Latino elders (HABLE) study methods and participant characteristics
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2021
;
13
(
1
):
e12202
.
27.
Institute for translational research
. (cited 2022 Sep 02). Available from: https://apps.unthsc.edu/itr/
28.
Zuelsdorff
M
,
Larson
JL
,
Hunt
JFV
,
Kim
AJ
,
Koscik
RL
,
Buckingham
WR
, et al
.
The area deprivation index: a novel tool for harmonizable risk assessment in Alzheimer’s disease research
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2020
;
6
(
1
):
e12039
.
29.
Bowie
CR
,
Harvey
PD
.
Administration and interpretation of the trail making test
.
Nat Protoc
.
2006
;
1
(
5
):
2277
81
.
30.
Salthouse
TA
.
What cognitive abilities are involved in trail-making performance
.
Intelligence
.
2011
;
39
(
4
):
222
32
.
31.
Jaeger
J
.
Digit symbol substitution test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing
.
Jclin Psychopharmacol
.
2018
;
38
(
5
):
513
9
.
32.
Beres
CA
,
Baron
A
.
Improved digit symbol substitution by older women as a result of extended practice
.
J Gerontol
.
1981
;
36
(
5
):
591
7
.
33.
O’Bryant
SE
,
Edwards
M
,
Johnson
L
,
Hall
J
,
Gamboa
A
,
O’Jile
J
.
Texas Mexican American Adult Normative Studies: normative data for commonly used clinical neuropsychological measures for English: and Spanish-speakers
.
Dev Neuropsychol
.
2018
;
43
(
1
):
1
26
.
34.
Sheffield
KM
,
Peek
MK
.
Neighborhood context and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans: results from the Hispanic established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2009
;
169
(
9
):
1092
101
.
35.
Reardon
SF
,
Fox
L
,
Townsend
J
.
Neighborhood income composition by household race and income, 1990–2009
.
ANNALS Am Acad Polit Social Sci
.
2015
;
660
(
1
):
78
97
.
36.
Phelan
JC
,
Link
BG
.
Is racism a fundamental cause of inequalities in health
.
Annu Rev Sociol
.
2015
;
41
(
1
):
311
30
.
37.
Letang
SK
,
Lin
SS-H
,
Parmelee
PA
,
McDonough
IM
.
Ethnoracial disparities in cognition are associated with multiple socioeconomic status-stress pathways
.
Cogn Res Princ Implic
.
2021
;
6
(
1
):
64
.
38.
Ozier
EM
,
Taylor
VJ
,
Murphy
MC
.
The cognitive effects of experiencing and observing subtle racial discrimination
.
J Soc Issues
.
2019
;
75
(
4
):
1087
115
.
39.
Cassarino
M
,
Setti
A
.
Environment as “brain training”: a review of geographical and physical environmental influences on cognitive ageing
.
Ageing Res Rev
.
2015
;
23
(
Pt B
):
167
82
.
40.
Rosso
AL
,
Flatt
JD
,
Carlson
MC
,
Lovasi
GS
,
Rosano
C
,
Brown
AF
, et al
.
Neighborhood socioeconomic status and cognitive function in late life
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2016
;
183
(
12
):
1088
97
.
41.
Yaffe
K
,
Falvey
C
,
Harris
TB
,
Newman
A
,
Satterfield
S
,
Koster
A
, et al
.
Effect of socioeconomic disparities on incidence of dementia among biracial older adults: prospective study
.
BMJ
.
2013
;
347
:
f7051
.
42.
Nordstrom
CK
,
Diez Roux
AV
,
Jackson
SA
,
Gardin
J
;
Cardiovascular Health Study
.
The association of personal and neighborhood socioeconomic indicators with subclinical cardiovascular disease in an elderly cohort. The cardiovascular health study
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2004
;
59
(
10
):
2139
47
.
43.
Smith
JA
,
Zhao
W
,
Wang
X
,
Ratliff
SM
,
Mukherjee
B
,
Kardia
SL
, et al
.
Neighborhood characteristics influence DNA methylation of genes involved in stress response and inflammation: the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis
.
Epigenetics
.
2017
;
12
(
8
):
662
73
.
44.
Simons
RL
,
Lei
MK
,
Klopack
E
,
Beach
SRH
,
Gibbons
FX
,
Philibert
RA
.
The effects of social adversity, discrimination, and health risk behaviors on the accelerated aging of African Americans: further support for the weathering hypothesis
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2021
;
282
:
113169
.