Abstract
Background/Aims: To detect cognitive decline in older adults, measures of verbal fluency and verbal memory are widely used. Less is known about performance in these measures in younger persons or according to education level and gender. We investigated cognitive performance according to age, education and gender among cognitively healthy adults aged 30–100 years. Methods: The study population comprised 4,174 cognitively healthy persons participating in the nationally representative Finnish Health 2011 survey. Cognitive assessment included verbal fluency, word list memory, word list recall and word list savings from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease neuropsychological battery. Results: Total variance in the cognitive test performance explained by age, education and gender varied from 12.3 to 31.2%. A decreasing trend in cognitive performance existed in all subtests by advancing age, with differences appearing between 50 and 55 years. Persons with the highest-education level performed best for all measures. For the participants < 55 years, education explained part of the variance, while age and gender did not. Conclusions: When assessing cognition, age and education should be accounted for in more detail in research and clinical practice. Additionally, the cohort effect and its potential impact on the renewal cycle of future normative values for cognitive tests should be considered.
Introduction
Ageing is the main risk factor for dementia: for people aged 60 years and over, the prevalence of dementia increases exponentially by age [1]. Many studies have evaluated cognitive changes related to normal ageing. With normal ageing, the processing speed often slows down, which may have implications for a variety of cognitive domains [2]. Ageing may also impair memory retrieval [2] that is the ability to access newly learned information. Later-born cohorts may have better cognitive performance than earlier-born cohorts when investigated at the same age [3]. Therefore, in addition to age there is also a need to investigate the association between age and cognition in newer cohorts.
Besides ageing, different demographic characteristics are related to cognitive performance. Higher education is associated with a lower risk of dementia [4-7]. Among persons with higher education, educational attainment may increase cognitive reserve; consequently, more age-related brain changes or Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology can accumulate before the manifestation of cognitive symptoms [8]. Additionally, it has been proposed that among persons with higher education, subjective and objective cognitive test evaluations can be used to predict future dementia well, but lower education makes this prediction more difficult [9].
In some regions, such as Western Europe, the prevalence of dementia is lower for men than women [1]. Gender differences in cognition appear to vary across birth cohorts and regions; the differences are associated with changes in living conditions and educational opportunities that different cohorts are exposed to during their formative years [10].
The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) was established to standardize and validate measures for the assessment of AD. The CERAD neuropsychological battery (CERAD-nb) was developed to measure early cognitive impairment in AD [11] and it is widely used in different countries and languages [12]. The CERAD-nb was translated into Finnish in 1999 [13]. Finnish normative values based on a random sample of 321 cognitively normal older adults (aged 63–79) were published in 2007 [14]; later publications included cutoff scores having sensitivity and specificity considerations [15, 16].
Verbal fluency tasks measure verbal production, semantic memory and language [17]. Semantic fluency deficits typically appear in persons with AD [18] or amnestic mild cognitive impairment [19]. The verbal fluency task in CERAD-nb (VFA) utilizes semantic animal category fluency [11]. Several recent studies have suggested that age [14, 20-23] and education [14, 20-24] account for much variance in VFA performance. Few studies, however, link gender to VFA [24, 25], with most studies showing no association [14, 20, 22].
Verbal memory measures are used to discriminate normal aging from AD and mild cognitive impairment [11, 26-29]. To examine these characteristics, the CERAD-nb uses word list memory (WLM), word list recall (WLR), word list saving (WLS) and word list recognition [11, 30]. WLR is usually the best overall CERAD-nb measure to identify patients with early AD [26-28]. Previous studies link higher education and lower age with better verbal memory performance [14, 24, 25]; for gender, however, results are inconsistent [14, 25]. Recent discoveries in WLM and WLR performance also include a cohort effect, with improvements between the earliest- and latest-born cohorts [31].
Many studies report data on the efficiency of verbal fluency and verbal memory measures in detecting cognitive decline in older adults [26-30]. Few studies have included younger participants. One study showed VFA performance linearly declines with age from early life [20], and another study showed that people with higher levels of education or younger age attain better VFA scores [22]. Little is known about population-based performance differences in these tests within a wider age range, or with a sufficiently large sample to cover different education levels. Normative data with wider age and education range are needed [32]. For use in clinical practice, population-based samples may give more meaningful reference values than smaller selected samples [24]. Our study aims to evaluate cognitive performance according to age, education and gender in a large nationally representative sample of cognitively healthy adults aged 30–100 years.
Material and Methods
Participants
The study data derived from the Finnish nationwide population-based health examination survey (Health 2011), which was carried out in 2011 and included adults aged 18 or older. Details of the design, implementation and results of the Health 2011 survey have been previously reported [33, 34]. The Health 2011 survey protocol was approved by the Coordinating Ethics Committee at the Hospital District of Helsinki and Uusimaa (reference 45/13/03/00/11). All participants provided their written informed consent.
Invited to the Health 2011 survey were 7,964 participants aged 30 or older. These participants had partaken in a similar national Health 2000 survey 11 years earlier. Of these persons, 4,664 participated in the health examination. Altogether, 4,544 persons were assessed using the CERAD-nb verbal fluency and verbal memory measures. In addition, the health interview involving participants aged 55 or older included a Six-Item Screener (SIS) that has been validated as a short version of the Mini Mental State Examination [35, 36]. Participant exclusion from the current analyses related to: incomplete CERAD-nb information n = 41; missing information on education n = 36; conditions affecting cognitive performance (stroke, psychosis, Parkinson’s, AD, vascular cognitive impairment, substance abuse, intellectual disability, encephalopathy) altogether n = 214; and potential dementia (SIS score less than 3) n = 79. Consequently, the final sample for our study comprised a total of 4,174 participants.
Information on self-reported education was categorized into three levels [33]. The lowest- education level included persons with no vocational training (beyond a vocational course or job training) and who had not taken a matriculation examination. The middle-education level included persons who had completed vocational school (regardless of basic education), and those who had passed a matricular examination but had no vocational training. The highest- education level included persons with degrees from higher vocational institutions, polytechnics and universities.
For the younger generations, age was categorized into 10-year age groups. For participants 55 years or older, age was categorized into 5-year age groups, as the risk for dementia after 60 years doubles every 6.5 years in Western and Central Europe [1]. The whole sample was divided into two subsamples: participants under 55 years (the group < 55) (n = 1,989), and participants over 55 years (the group ≥55) (n = 2,185). Creation of the latter group enabled comparisons with other studies focusing on older people [14-16, 23-25, 30].
Procedures and Measurements
Participants of the Health 2011 attended a comprehensive health examination and health interview given by trained nurses in a health center near each participant’s place of residence [33, 34]. The concise examination was performed at home if the participant was unable to attend the health examination site. Selected parts of the CERAD battery, namely VFA, WLM and WLR, were included in the study protocol. In the VFA, the participant was asked to generate as many examples of animals as possible in 60 s. The WLM task included three trials to learn a list of 10 unrelated words presented each time in a different order. The participant read the words aloud and was asked to recall as many words as possible at the end of each learning trial. The recalled words from each trial were summed to produce the WLM score. After VFA and WLM tasks, a handgrip test was administered before WLR. The WLR part of the test was presented 5 min after the third learning trial. The participant was asked to recall as many words as possible. The WLS score was calculated as a percentage of WLR to learning trial 3 in WLM.
Statistical Analyses
The analyses were conducted using SPSS complex samples add-on software, which accounts for sampling design. Weighting the observations produced estimates representative of the Finnish adult population by reducing the nonresponse bias [33, 34].
Differences in cognitive performance between education levels, age groups, and genders, along with interactions between these variables, were evaluated by analyses of variance (ANOVA). Evaluation of the effects involved the ANOVA model effect, R2 of each analysis and a combination of each. Performance of pairwise comparisons used Bonferroni correction. A p value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant, and corrected p values were used throughout the study. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 24.0 or 25.0 with complex sample add-on (IBM Corp. Released 2015; IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, version 24.0; Armonk, NY, IBM Corp.).
Results
Table 1 presents the number of participants by age, education and gender (56% women), and Table 2 presents their education in years of formal education.
Table 3 provides detailed descriptive statistics for all measured cognitive tasks by age, education and gender. Additionally, Figure 1 visualizes the overall performance.
For the whole sample, the overall mean for VFA was 24.9 words. Participants with higher education performed better than those with lower education (p < 0.001), and younger participants outperformed older participants (p < 0.001) (Table 3 and Fig. 1). The main effect of gender was not significant (p = 0.893); however, interaction existed between education and gender (p = 0.040): among those with the highest education, men performed better than women.
The overall mean of WLM was 21.8 words. Participants with higher education performed better than those with lower education (p < 0.001), and younger participants outperformed older participants (p < 0.001) (Table 3 and Fig. 1). Further, women outperformed men (p < 0.001). A three-way interaction existed between education, age and gender (p = 0.006).
The overall mean for WLR was 7.6 words. Participants with higher education performed better than those with lower education (p < 0.001), younger participants outperformed older participants (p < 0.001), and women outperformed men (p < 0.001). The overall mean for WLS was 88.1%. Also, WLS participants with higher education performed better than those with lower education (p < 0.001), younger participants outperformed older participants (p < 0.001) (Fig. 1 and Table 3), and women outperformed men (p < 0.001). Age and education interacted regarding WLS (p = 0.020), as the age effect was more pronounced with lower levels of education.
For the whole sample, the total variance explained by age, education and gender was largest for WLM (31.2%) and WLR (28.2%). For VFA, it was 15.8% and for WLS 12.3%. For the group ≥55, the effect sizes were similar to the whole sample; however, for the group < 55 demographic variables explained substantially less of the variance in cognitive performance (Table 4).
For the whole sample, age accounted for the highest proportion of variance for WLR at 11.3% and WLM at 11.2%, and less for VFA at 4.3% and WLS at 4.8%. Age had a small performance effect in the group < 55.
In the whole sample, the Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons between age groups indicated that some groups did not significantly differ from each other; subsequently, we created combined age groups. For VFA, the combined age groups could be summarized into 30–49 years, 50–69 years, 70–74 years and ≥75 years, for WLM into 30–54, 55–64 years, 65–74 years and ≥75 years, for WLR into 30–49, 50–54, 55–64 years, 65–74 years and ≥75 years and for WLS into 30–49, 50–54, 55–74 years and ≥75 years.
Education accounted for 5.1% of variance for VFA, 5.0% for WLM, 3.5% for WLR and 1.5% for WLS. In the group < 55, education accounted for more variance than age or gender (Table 4). In the whole sample, the Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons between education levels indicated the lowest- and middle-education levels did not differ for VFA and WLS. For VFA and WLS, education levels were combined. The highest-education level differed (p < 0.001) for all cognitive outcomes. To clarify this, cognitive performance according to grouping of both age and education strata is presented in Figure 2.
Gender had only a minor effect on cognitive performance.
Discussion
We report on the scope of performance in both verbal fluency and verbal memory measures for a large representative sample of Finnish cognitively healthy participants aged between 30 and 100 years. Results from these cognitive tasks from the CERAD-nb indicated a decreasing trend in performance by advancing age, with differences beginning from the age of 50 or 55. For all used cognitive measures, persons with highest-education levels performed better than other educational groups. Persons with middle-education levels performed similarly to those with lowest-education levels regarding verbal fluency and verbal memory savings. For the group < 55, education was the only demographic variable impacting cognitive performance.
For the whole sample, the total effect size of demographic variables on performance was largest for WLM (31.2%) and WLR (28.2%), less for VFA (15.8%) and least for WLS (12.3%). Age affected performance for all variables, but not uniformly: for the group < 55, age had only minimal effects on performance.
The mean level of cognitive performance of the group ≥55 in our study was better than in previously published Finnish normative values [14-16]: VFA by roughly +2 words, WLM by +0.5 words, WLS by +1% (WLR was previously not published), and the mean of education was 1.5 years higher. Additionally, the effect of age in our study was remarkably stronger for VFA and WLM, but less for WLS. The effect of education was almost the same as previously; for WLS, however, a new mild effect was found. The effect of gender was clearly smaller in our study. These differences between our study and the previous Finnish study [14, 37] might reflect both ongoing changes in education levels of the Finnish population and the higher proportion of educated women in the group ≥55 [38]. This agrees with the cohort effects in cognition described in recent studies [3, 10, 31].
All variables indicated a noticeable effect of education on cognitive performance. We found a clear effect of education as the highest level differed from other levels for all cognitive measures. This result contrasts with the original CERAD-nb normative study by Welsh et al. [30] which indicated no differences between low and highly educated people on VFA, WLR and WLS. The small number of lower-educated participants in the original CERAD-nb norms [30] may partly explain this difference. For the group ≥55, the age effect of VFA also differed from the original CERAD-nb norms that indicated a lack of age effect for both highly and lower educated groups. For verbal memory measures, the group ≥55 had a similar decrease in performance with age, with women scoring better as in the original study. In our study, WLS showed a similar mild [30] but significant effect of age but also indicated a mild effect of education.
Our results are in line with some other earlier studies, which found that both age [14, 20-23, 25] and education [14, 20-25] affected VFA. In some studies, the magnitude of the age effect has been larger [22], while in others it has been closer to the 4% observed in our study [14, 23, 25] except for one [24] which agrees with the original study with no age effect found [30]. The effect of education on VFA has been in several earlier studies [14, 22, 23, 25] close to our 5% finding. Gender had no independent effect on VFA in our study, which is in line with most previous studies [14, 20-22] but contrasts to one recent population-based study [24].
Our results of WLM, WLR and WLS are in line with a recent population-based study [24] with younger or more educated persons or women performing better. When comparing WLM and WLR to effect sizes of the elderly-focused large international study by Sosa et al. [25], the group ≥55 had a similar education-level effect but the age effect in our study was almost double. Though the gender effect in our study was mild, it was 6 (WLR) to 9 (WLM) times larger than in the study of Sosa et al. [25], who found effects of only 0.6 and 0.5%.
Strengths and Limitations
We used several measures of cognitive performance from data of a large, nationwide population-based study. The observations were weighted to produce estimates that represent the Finnish adult population by reducing the nonresponse bias. Due to health-related factors possibly affecting cognitive performance, medical exclusion criteria were used. The high prevalence of dementia in older age led to using a cognitive SIS to secure a cognitively healthy sample. The large sample size, large age range between 30 and 100, and coverage of all education levels are strengths of our study.
Various limitations are acknowledged in our study. First, performance may include a practice effect, as the participants had been assessed with the same tests 11 years earlier. Second, some early-state dementia participants may have been included in our study despite the use of SIS. Third, notwithstanding the large total sample, small group numbers existed for the number of persons in the lowest education level of age group 30–49, and the weighted number of highest-education level participants aged 70 and over. Consequently, these groups must be carefully interpreted. Fourth, conducting the WLR task after the handgrip test may produce slightly different results than using the traditional CERAD-nb protocol. Fifth, as for any population-based study, those who participate may have better health and cognitive function than nonparticipants.
Conclusion
In conclusion, our study on verbal fluency and verbal memory measures in a population-based sample of cognitively healthy participants aged between 30 and 100 years indicates that age differences begin from the age of 50 or 55. Furthermore, the highest-education level participants performed better than those with other education levels. The effect of demographic variables is smaller for the group < 55. For the group ≥55, age has the strongest impact on performance. Comparing our results to the previously published Finnish normative reveals a potential cohort effect. Future studies and clinical use of cognitive tasks to detect dementia require renewed and enlarged normative values for the group ≥55. Also necessary is consideration of the cohort effect and its potential impact on the renewal cycle of future normative values.
Acknowledgments
We thank all of the Health 2011 participants and members of the Health 2011 study group for their cooperation, and data collection and management.
Statement of Ethics
The Health 2011 survey protocol was approved by the Coordinating Ethics Committee at the Hospital District of Helsinki and Uusimaa (reference 45/13/03/00/11). All participants provided their written informed consent.
Disclosure Statement
There are no conflicts of interest.