Background: Research on person-centered cognitive testing is beginning to emerge. The current study is the first to focus on eliciting concrete preferences around the test experience. Methods: Adults ≥50 years old completed the Attitudes Around Cognitive Testing (AACT) questionnaire on mturk.com. AACT elicits preferences for cognitive tests, the importance attributed to having choices, and willingness to engage in testing. Results: Data are reported for 289 respondents. The proportion of participants expressing preferences varied by domain (modality [49.5%], location [47.2%], company [80.1%], result delivery [78.3–89.7%]). Importance ratings for all domains had a median of 4 and a range of 1–5 using a Likert scale of agreement. Most participants (85.5%) were willing to engage in testing. Conclusion: Older adults have preferences for cognitive tests, especially with delivery of results.

The importance of detecting cognitive impairment in older adults during primary care annual wellness visits has been recognized by gerontology and geriatrics experts [1], stakeholder associations [2], and policy makers [2, 3]. Despite this, more than 50% of persons with dementia have never received a diagnosis from a physician [4]. Currently, the detection of cognitive impairment and dementia is based on case finding. Screening is initiated by clinician’s suspicion, and a diagnosis is made after the clinician conducts further tests or refers the patient for imaging and full diagnostic assessment [5]. A survey of primary care providers found that following suspicion of cognitive impairment, the majority of physicians conduct medical tests to exclude underlying causes, assess for depression and daily functioning, and perform a mental status test [6]. A systematic review and meta-analysis by Tsoi et al. [7] revealed large variations in the use of mental status tests physicians use to aid in the detection cognitive impairment; 11 screening tests were identified, ranging in administration time from 5 to 20 min, and varying in cognitive domains measured. For example, the widely used Mini-Mental State Examination measures orientation, memory, language, attention, and visuospatial abilities, while the Mini-Cog measures memory, visuospatial, and executive functioning.

Despite the recognized importance of detecting cognitive impairment in older adults, it is often missed in primary care [8]. Factors related to missed and delayed diagnosis of dementia in medical settings fall into four categories: physician, system-related, caregiver, and patient factors. Physician and system-related factors involve communication problems between the physician and patient and limited appointment time [9]. Caregiver factors include preferences towards not knowing the patients’ condition [9, 10]. Patient factors include older patient age, being unmarried, and having lower levels of education [9]. Patient refusal to be assessed or to be treated if diagnosed is also a commonly cited barrier. More than one-third (37.7%) of older primary care patients in one recent study refused diagnostic screening [11]. In another study, approximately half (47.7%) of the primary care patients who screened positive for dementia refused full diagnostic assessment [12]. Patient characteristics associated with the refusal of screening or follow-up diagnostic assessment include the absence of subjective concern of cognitive symptoms [12], living alone, and reported distress about the possibility of being diagnosed with dementia [13]. The subjective experience of cognitive testing is also important in assessing older adults’ participation in screening and the diagnostic process. Older adults have described cognitive tests as stressful, bewildering, and embarrassing [4, 14]. They may perceive tests as a threat to their dignity and self-respect [14]. The subjective experience of distress related to cognitive testing may also be a function of increasing cognitive impairment, and predictors of distress may be related to patient awareness about test difficulty and performance [15].

To date, few studies have investigated how to reduce patient-level barriers to dementia detection. Research utilizing a person-centered approach to overcome such barriers is an unmet need. In person-centered care, healthcare providers explore patients’ preferences and attitudes around their care, and also provide them with information to help them make decisions regarding diagnostic and therapeutic interventions [16, 17]. Preferences are viewed as a “tendency to consider something desirable or undesirable” [18] and attitudes are a “set of beliefs, feelings, and behavioral tendencies towards a socially significant event or object” [19]. Research on person-centered care provides evidence that such an approach to healthcare improves objective and subjective health outcomes [20]. The core features of person-centered care include exploring patient preferences and increasing patient autonomy to promote self-control and self-efficacy [21, 22], and to enable more effective participation in the healthcare system [23].

Attaining person-centered care may be particularly important for older adults given this population’s heterogeneity in health and level of functioning, personal treatment preferences, and individual goals. A recent study found that older adults prefer to participate actively in healthcare decisions [24]. More understanding is needed regarding how older adults can be empowered to engage in their healthcare decisions. In the context of cognitive testing, one approach to person-centered testing may include inviting older adults to make choices regarding their experience (e.g., where they take the test, who would be with them during the test, the modality in which the test is taken, how they receive their results). In turn, such choices may lead to greater acceptance of the cognitive test experience and its results.

The goal of the present study was to elicit older adults’ attitudes and preferences towards cognitive testing. The Attitudes Around Cognitive Testing (AACT) questionnaire was de veloped specifically for the current study to identify attitudes and preferences regarding cognitive testing in the context of a visit with ones’ primary care physician. Our aim was to characterize older adults’ preferences regarding the cognitive test experience, and to probe the importance they attach to making choices according to their preferences. We also examined the association between importance ratings and willingness to undergo cognitive testing. We hypothesized that older adults will value having choices regarding the cognitive test experience. We also hypothesized that older adults will be more willing to undergo cognitive testing if they can choose the circumstances of their test experience.

The methods are reported in accordance with the Checklist for reporting Results of Internet Surveys [25].

Participants

Adults 50 years or older and fluent in English were recruited through an online crowdsourcing site, Amazon Mechanical Turk (www.mturk.com). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk is a web-based market where requesters post jobs and workers choose which jobs to do for pay. Workers on Mechanical Turk browse amongst existing jobs and are not under any obligation to complete particular tasks. Initial contact with eligible participants was made online. Data collection relied on an open survey format, allowing any potential respondents who met eligibility criteria to voluntarily partake in the study. Accordingly, AACT was not particularly promoted to eligible respondents. Eligibility criteria for MTurk users required participants to (1) be registered on MTurk with a birthdate before 1967 to ensure age requirements were met (at least 50 years of age), (2) be geographically located in the United States, and (3) have provided valid, acceptable data on at least 95% of previously competed MTurk surveys/tasks. Before consenting to the study, respondents were informed of the purpose of the study, the risks and benefits of the research, and the contact information to study personnel should they experience problems or concerns in the course of participating in the study. Due to the anonymous nature of the questionnaire, no identifying information was collected or stored. Before submitting responses, respondents were able to review and change their answers. Only one response per IP address was accepted. Upon completion of the questionnaire, participants were paid USD 1. Data were collected from December 2017 through January 2018, concluding once 300 responses were collected.

The study received approval from the Pacific University Institutional Review Board.

Measures

AACT items captured sociodemographic and background characteristics, personal preferences around cognitive tests, importance of being given choices around the cognitive test experience, and willingness to undergo cognitive testing. We developed AACT items based on multiple studies that have elicited patients’ attitudes and preferences in the context of their medical care [17, 26-29]. We focused on preferences in five domains of the cognitive test experience: (1) modality, (2) location, (3) company, (4) result delivery for negative results (cognitive test indicates no changes in the participants’ memory or thinking), and (5) result delivery for positive results (cognitive test indicates changes in the participants’ memory or thinking). In order to assess the value of being given choices in relation to participants’ preferences, we used the term “importance of choosing” throughout AACT. Finally, we evaluated participants’ willingness to undergo cognitive testing drawing on existing measures and attitudinal surveys of dementia screening [10, 30]. The response format varies throughout AACT, including 5-point Likert scale of agreement, binary choices of yes or no, and selecting all options that apply. Throughout AACT, we used the term Alzheimer’s disease as opposed to dementia because previous work by Boustani et al. [10] suggested that “Alzheimer’s disease” is more readily understood. We provided participants with the general context of taking a test that measures memory and thinking with their doctor and asked them about their thoughts and feelings. We decided to avoid distinguishing between different types of cognitive tests (e.g., cognitive screening tests, neuropsychological testing) to eliminate potential confusion among respondents. Sample items illustrating our three primary constructs of preferences, importance, and willingness are provided in Table 1. The instructions provided to orient respondents to the context of the survey are also shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Examples of items in AACT evaluating primary outcomes of preferences, importance of choice, and willingness to undergo cognitive testing

Examples of items in AACT evaluating primary outcomes of preferences, importance of choice, and willingness to undergo cognitive testing
Examples of items in AACT evaluating primary outcomes of preferences, importance of choice, and willingness to undergo cognitive testing

Statistical Analysis

Participants’ preferences were captured in a “select all that apply format,” and coded into different categories. Those in the “No preference category” selected only “I have no preferences,” and no other response. Those in a “Single preference” category endorsed one specific preference respective to the cognitive test domain in question. Those in the “Multiple preference” category endorsed more than one preference. Finally, we coded invalid responses as those that endorsed “I have no preferences” and one or more specific preferences, and excluded invalid responses from our analyses. For additional analyses, preferences responses were recoded a second time to reflect a dichotomous preference variable, differentiating between those who endorsed preferences and those who did not endorse preferences. Participants’ Likert-scale ratings were reported with median and range as measures of central tendency and variability, as recommended by Sullivan and Artino [31]. Willingness was modeled as a binary variable: Willing included those that strongly agreed or agreed to undergoing testing; Not willing included those that were neutral, disagreed or strongly disagreed.

All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS statistical software (25.0.0.0). The statistical significance was assumed at p < 0.05. We used the χ2 test to assess whether dichotomized preference responses (any or no preference) were associated with person characteristics including gender, age, education level, and subjective cognitive concern. We used nonparametric Mann-Whitney U tests to identify associations between importance ratings and the categorical person characteristics. We used binary logistic regression to examine the associations between willingness to undergo cognitive testing, preferences, and importance ratings.

Participant Characteristics

All submitted questionnaires were completed. Data were available for 300 respondents and analyzed for 289 participants who completed AACT. Individuals who endorsed a previous diagnosis of a neurocognitive disorder were excluded from our analyses. Missing data were excluded from analyses at the item level. No clear pattern of missing data emerged. Participant characteristics are provided in Table 2. The overall age of participants ranged from 50 to 80 at the time of survey completion (mean = 63 years), with the majority being between 50 and 65 years of age (69%). Most participants were female (66.7%) and White (90.4%). Nearly half of the participants did not have a college degree (51.9%) and were not married (51.5%). More than half of the participants endorsed subjective cognitive concern (63.2%). Most of our respondents reported no previous experience with cognitive tests (89.4%), and/or no diagnosis of neurocognitive disorder (99%).

Table 2.

Characteristics of study participants who completed online versions of AACT

Characteristics of study participants who completed online versions of AACT
Characteristics of study participants who completed online versions of AACT

Preferences regarding the Cognitive Test Experience

Participants’ preferences regarding the five domains of the cognitive test experience are presented in Table 3. The proportion of participants expressing preferences varied by domain and ranged from 49.5% for modality to 89.7% for delivery of concerning results. The most frequently endorsed single preferences were to take the test on a computer or a mobile device (27.0%), at home (33.4%), and alone (62.5%), for modality, location, and company, respectively. Most participants had preferences regarding result delivery. The most frequently endorsed singular preference was a telephone call (17.9%) for receiving negative (normal) results, and through an office visit with their physician for positive (concerning) results (32.4%).

Table 3.

Participants’ preferences for the five domains of cognitive testing

Participants’ preferences for the five domains of cognitive testing
Participants’ preferences for the five domains of cognitive testing

Female gender and subjective cognitive concern were associated with a higher proportion of those endorsing preferences. Specifically, women tended to report having more preferences for how they receive negative results (women: 55.0%, men: 23.5%, χ2 [1, n = 251] = 4.02, p < 0.05) and positive results (women: 62.7%, men: 26.9%; χ2 [1, n = 260] = 5.26, p < 0.05) than men. Those with subjective cognitive concerns reported having preferences more than those without concerns for the delivery of positive results only (subjective concern: 58.4%, no subjective concern: 31.3%; χ2 [1, n = 262] = 4.43, p < 0.05).

Importance of Being Given Choices

The distribution of respondents’ perceived importance of being given choices regarding cognitive testing for each domain is provided in Table 4. Higher values for the 5-point Likert ratings represented higher levels of agreement with importance. Generally, importance ratings were high for all domains, with a median of 4, and a range of 1–5.

Table 4.

Self-perceived importance of having choices in the testing experience using a 5-point Likert scale of agreement

Self-perceived importance of having choices in the testing experience using a 5-point Likert scale of agreement
Self-perceived importance of having choices in the testing experience using a 5-point Likert scale of agreement

Nonparametric Mann-Whitney tests indicated that those who endorsed preferences in a given test domain also gave higher importance ratings in that domain compared to those who did not have preferences. The median importance ratings of those with preferences were 1–2 points higher than those with no preferences in all domains of cognitive testing (modality [p = 0.00]; location [p = 0.00]; company [p = 0.00]; negative result delivery [p = 0.00]; positive result delivery [p = 0.00]). Female gender was associated with higher importance ratings. Specifically, women gave higher ratings for choosing the location (p = 0.03), company (p = 0.00), and result delivery (p = 0.00) than men.

Willingness

The majority of respondents were willing to engage in cognitive testing (85.5%), with a small proportion reporting they were ambivalent (10.5%) or in disagreement to testing (4.0%). None of the person variables was associated with willingness.

Results of binary logistic regressions predicting willingness to engage in cognitive testing are summarized in Table 5. The variable that predicted willingness to engage in cognitive testing was importance of choosing location. Lower perceived importance of choosing the location of the cognitive test predicts willingness to engage in cognitive testing (p = 0.04).

Table 5.

Summary of binary logistic regression model results to predict willingness to engage in cognitive testing

Summary of binary logistic regression model results to predict willingness to engage in cognitive testing
Summary of binary logistic regression model results to predict willingness to engage in cognitive testing

Sparse literature exists on older adults’ attitudes towards cognitive tests [11, 32]. The present study is the first to elicit older adults’ specific preferences in five domains of the cognitive test experience, the value they place in being given choices in those five domains, and their willingness to undergo cognitive testing. Overall, we found support for our hypotheses that older adults have preferences regarding the test-taking experience and they attach substantial importance to these preferences. Women and those with subjective cognitive concerns were more likely to endorse preferences in multiple domains. Somewhat surprisingly, most participants were willing to take part in cognitive testing, and their willingness was generally not associated with having preferences and valuing choices.

About half of respondents had preferences for modality and location, whereas a large majority endorsed preferences regarding company. Respondents also gave high importance ratings for having choices in these domains of cognitive testing. A frequently endorsed combination of preferences was to take the cognitive test on a computer or mobile device, at home, and alone. This preference pattern provides support for the use of telehealth to administer cognitive tests. Telehealth is a system of technology that allows for the remote provision of medical services using videoconferencing technologies and provides an alternative medium for cognitive assessment. Telehealth is thus a means that could potentially satisfy the most popular endorsed preferences in this study, allowing older adults to take diagnostically informative tests at home, by themselves, and with a computer or mobile device. While the reliability and validity of cognitive assessments via telehealth remains to be fully established, this approach promises utility in the assessment of cognitive functioning among older adults in rural, underserved areas [33, 34].

The most sensitive domain was delivery of results, where a large majority of respondents had specific preferences and placed unequivocal value on having choices in the matter. Preferences were contingent on the type of results, with most people preferring to receive negative results through a telephone call and positive results through an office visit. The high value placed on preferences and choices with regard to test results suggest that it may be of particular importance for healthcare professionals to educate patients about cognitive tests and prepare them for the results they produce, before undertaking testing. At that time, the provider should also explore patients’ preferences regarding the communication of results. Our finding is consistent with other findings suggesting that accommodating patient preferences for the delivery of test results may improve patient-physician communication [35]. It is likely that the responses we obtained in the test result domain may have captured broader concerns regarding the disclosure of results not assessed in the AACT items. These aspects may encompass patients’ need to understand the implications of the results, how results might impact their lives, what decisions need to be made next, and how to share information with family members. Our findings also indicate that the respondents who are the most concerned about the delivery of nonnormal results were those with subjective cognitive concerns. It has been previously reported that only a minority of those with subjective memory complaints seek help for medical concerns [36, 37], even though this group is at an increased risk of developing cognitive impairment or dementia [38]. It may be that the fear of receiving positive results contributes to the apprehension those with subjective cognitive concerns experience in seeking medical help. Probing for subjective concerns and addressing the fear of positive results may also need to be part of the communication that prepares patients for cognitive testing.

An intriguing association emerged between gender, likelihood of having preferences, and the value placed on having choices. Women were more likely to endorse preferences for how they receive negative and positive results of cognitive testing and reported it more important to be given choices around the location, company, and mode of result delivery. These associations suggest women may be more invested in the experience of taking diagnostically informative cognitive tests, further contributing to the literature on the gender disparities seen in engagement with healthcare services. Literature spanning several decades point to men being less likely to use physician services and preventive healthcare measures than women [39], possibly due to men delaying help seeking in the context of their health [39, 40]. The gender differences reported in the current study suggest women may be especially responsive to a person-centered approach to cognitive testing. It remains unknown if proactively involving men in the decision making process would be likely to increase their engagement with utilization of preventive care services, including cognitive screening for the detection of cognitive impairment and dementia. Further investigation into gender and its relations to person-centered care and consequential healthcare utilization is warranted.

While a recent study found that high behavioral intention to undergo screening is positively related to participation in screening for cognitive impairment [41], it is important to note AACT’s measurement of willingness focused on perceived willingness rather than actual testing acceptance behaviors. We expected older adults to exhibit variability in terms of their willingness to engage in cognitive testing. In our sample, the majority of our respondents (85.5%) were willing to engage in cognitive testing. This percentage is much higher than previous estimates of acceptance of cognitive screening or assessment (49 and 52.3%) [12, 30]. The atypically high endorsement of willingness in our sample may explain why we did not find a robust pattern of associations between this variable, endorsed preferences, and importance ratings. The association we found between lower perceived importance of choosing the location of the cognitive test and higher willingness to engage in cognitive tests is difficult to interpret in isolation. Data from samples drawn from actual health settings may clarify the relationship between patients’ attitudes and behaviors, and ultimately establish the benefits of centering testing around patients’ preferences. Such data could include intervention studies that allowed opportunities for older adults to make choices around the cognitive text experience, and then examining the association between opportunity of choice and willingness to engage in future testing.

There are other limitations to consider in interpreting our findings. First, participants were online survey respondents who self-selected to participate in our study and may represent an opportunistic sample. Further, our respondents were generally younger than 65 years old, and thus our sample may not be representative of the older adults in the primary care population. Accordingly, our results should be confirmed in samples of older adults attending primary care settings. Research concerning data collection using Mechanical Turk indicate that data obtained through this online platform are at least as reliable as those obtained via traditional methods, and that the platform serves as effective and valid tool for behavioral research [42]. However, we recognize that the pattern of endorsed preferences, particularly preferences supporting a telehealth approach, may be specific to MTurk workers. An additional limitation includes AACT’s focus on five pragmatic domains of the test-taking experience. With this emphasis on the concrete circumstances of testing, we may have missed dimensions of the experience that matter to older adults. The AACT questionnaire was developed for this study with largely exploratory aims, and its psychometric properties have not been established. The constructs of attitudes and specific preferences for cognitive tests have not been measured in previous research. Thus, our primary aim was to provide novel information, not to develop a standardized measure. Finally, we did not ask our respondents about their attitudes and preferences for specific types of cognitive testing currently in use (i.e., screening, neuropsychological testing). This distinction is likely important for formulating person-centered approaches and should be investigated in future studies.

In summary, our study provides the first evidence of its kind that opportunities exist for developing a person-centered approach for cognitive testing among older adults. Such opportunities include asking older adults their preferences for the parameters in which they take diagnostically informative tests. It appears that older adults value choices in regard to the testing situation and have variability in preferences for the manner in which they wish to take cognitive tests. A suggested earlier, it remains to be proven that offering choices in the context of cognitive testing, including the use of telehealth and thorough discussions around discussions of results, would lead to better outcomes around cognitive assessment.

All respondents provided informed consent prior to the study. The study protocol was approved by the Pacific University Institutional Review Board.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest in relation to this study. Both authors have read the paper and have agreed to be listed as authors.

1.
Morley
JE
,
Morris
JC
,
Berg-Weger
M
,
Borson
S
,
Carpenter
BD
,
Del Campo
N
, et al
Brain health: the importance of recognizing cognitive impairment: an IAGG consensus conference
.
J Am Med Dir Assoc
.
2015
Sep
;
16
(
9
):
731
9
.
[PubMed]
1525-8610
2.
Cordell
CB
,
Borson
S
,
Boustani
M
,
Chodosh
J
,
Reuben
D
,
Verghese
J
, et al;
Medicare Detection of Cognitive Impairment Workgroup
.
Alzheimer’s Association recommendations for operationalizing the detection of cognitive impairment during the Medicare Annual Wellness Visit in a primary care setting
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2013
Mar
;
9
(
2
):
141
50
.
[PubMed]
1552-5260
3.
Lin
JS
,
O’Connor
E
,
Rossom
RC
,
Perdue
LA
,
Eckstrom
E
.
Screening for cognitive impairment in older adults: A systematic review for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force
.
Ann Intern Med
.
2013
Nov
;
159
(
9
):
601
12
.
[PubMed]
1539-3704
4.
Martin
S
,
Kelly
S
,
Khan
A
,
Cullum
S
,
Dening
T
,
Rait
G
, et al
Attitudes and preferences towards screening for dementia: a systematic review of the literature
.
BMC Geriatr
.
2015
Jun
;
15
(
1
):
66
.
[PubMed]
1471-2318
5.
Brayne
C
,
Fox
C
,
Boustani
M
.
Dementia screening in primary care: is it time?
JAMA
.
2007
Nov
;
298
(
20
):
2409
11
.
[PubMed]
0098-7484
6.
Cody
M
,
Beck
C
,
Shue
VM
,
Pope
S
.
Reported practices of primary care physicians in the diagnosis and management of dementia
.
Aging Ment Health
.
2002
Feb
;
6
(
1
):
72
6
.
[PubMed]
1360-7863
7.
Tsoi
KK
,
Chan
JY
,
Hirai
HW
,
Wong
SY
,
Kwok
TC
.
Cognitive Tests to Detect Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis
.
JAMA Intern Med
.
2015
Sep
;
175
(
9
):
1450
8
.
[PubMed]
2168-6106
8.
Borson
S
,
Frank
L
,
Bayley
PJ
,
Boustani
M
,
Dean
M
,
Lin
PJ
, et al
Improving dementia care: the role of screening and detection of cognitive impairment
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2013
Mar
;
9
(
2
):
151
9
.
[PubMed]
1552-5260
9.
Bradford
A
,
Kunik
ME
,
Schulz
P
,
Williams
SP
,
Singh
H
.
Missed and delayed diagnosis of dementia in primary care: prevalence and contributing factors
.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
.
2009
Oct-Dec
;
23
(
4
):
306
14
.
[PubMed]
0893-0341
10.
Boustani
MA
,
Justiss
MD
,
Frame
A
,
Austrom
MG
,
Perkins
AJ
,
Cai
X
, et al
Caregiver and noncaregiver attitudes toward dementia screening
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2011
Apr
;
59
(
4
):
681
6
.
[PubMed]
0002-8614
11.
Fowler
NR
,
Perkins
AJ
,
Turchan
HA
,
Frame
A
,
Monahan
P
,
Gao
S
, et al
Older primary care patients’ attitudes and willingness to screen for dementia
.
J Aging Res
.
2015
;
2015
:
423265
.
[PubMed]
2090-2204
12.
Boustani
M
,
Perkins
AJ
,
Fox
C
,
Unverzagt
F
,
Austrom
MG
,
Fultz
B
, et al
Who refuses the diagnostic assessment for dementia in primary care?
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2006
Jun
;
21
(
6
):
556
63
.
[PubMed]
0885-6230
13.
Fowler
NR
,
Frame
A
,
Perkins
AJ
,
Gao
S
,
Watson
DP
,
Monahan
P
, et al
Traits of patients who screen positive for dementia and refuse diagnostic assessment
.
Alzheimers Dement (Amst)
.
2015
Jun
;
1
(
2
):
236
41
.
[PubMed]
2352-8729
14.
Krohne
K
,
Slettebø
A
,
Bergland
A
.
Cognitive screening tests as experienced by older hospitalised patients: a qualitative study
.
Scand J Caring Sci
.
2011
Dec
;
25
(
4
):
679
87
.
[PubMed]
0283-9318
15.
Lai
JM
,
Hawkins
KA
,
Gross
CP
,
Karlawish
JH
.
Self-reported distress after cognitive testing in patients with Alzheimer’s disease
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
.
2008
Aug
;
63
(
8
):
855
9
.
[PubMed]
1079-5006
16.
American Geriatrics Society Expert Panel on Person-Centered Care
.
Person-centered care: A definition and essential elements
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2016
Jan
;
64
(
1
):
15
8
.
[PubMed]
0002-8614
17.
Fraenkel
L
.
Incorporating patients’ preferences into medical decision making
.
Med Care Res Rev
.
2013
Feb
;
70
(
1
Suppl
):
80S
93S
.
[PubMed]
1077-5587
18.
Warren
C
,
McGraw
AP
,
Van Boven
L
.
Values and preferences: defining preference construction
.
Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci
.
2011
Mar
;
2
(
2
):
193
205
.
[PubMed]
1939-5078
19.
Hogg
MA
,
Vaughn
G
.
M. Essentials of Social Psychology
.
Essex, England
:
Pearson Education Limited
;
2010
.
20.
Olsson
LE
,
Jakobsson Ung
E
,
Swedberg
K
,
Ekman
I
.
Efficacy of person-centred care as an intervention in controlled trials - a systematic review
.
J Clin Nurs
.
2013
Feb
;
22
(
3-4
):
456
65
.
[PubMed]
0962-1067
21.
Epstein
RM
,
Fiscella
K
,
Lesser
CS
,
Stange
KC
.
Why the nation needs a policy push on patient-centered health care
.
Health Aff (Millwood)
.
2010
Aug
;
29
(
8
):
1489
95
.
[PubMed]
0278-2715
22.
O’Hair
D
,
Villagran
MM
,
Wittenberg
E
,
Brown
K
,
Ferguson
M
,
Hall
HT
, et al
Cancer survivorship and agency model: implications for patient choice, decision making, and influence
.
Health Commun
.
2003
;
15
(
2
):
193
202
.
[PubMed]
1041-0236
23.
Lee
YY
,
Lin
JL
.
Do patient autonomy preferences matter? Linking patient-centered care to patient-physician relationships and health outcomes
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2010
Nov
;
71
(
10
):
1811
8
.
[PubMed]
0277-9536
24.
Wolff
JL
,
Boyd
CM
.
A look at person-centered and family-centered care among older adults: results from a national survey
.
J Gen Intern Med
.
2015
;
30
(
10
):
1497
504
.
[PubMed]
0884-8734
25.
Eysenbach
G
.
Improving the quality of Web surveys: the Checklist for Reporting Results of Internet E-Surveys (CHERRIES)
.
J Med Internet Res
.
2004
Sep
;
6
(
3
):
e34
.
[PubMed]
1438-8871
26.
Fried
TR
,
Bradley
EH
,
Towle
VR
.
Assessment of patient preferences: integrating treatments and outcomes
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
2002
Nov
;
57
(
6
):
S348
54
.
[PubMed]
1079-5014
27.
LaVela
SL
,
Schectman
G
,
Gering
J
,
Locatelli
SM
,
Gawron
A
,
Weaver
FM
.
Understanding health care communication preferences of veteran primary care users
.
Patient Educ Couns
.
2012
Sep
;
88
(
3
):
420
6
.
[PubMed]
0738-3991
28.
Sidani
S
,
Epstein
D
,
Miranda
J
.
Eliciting patient treatment preferences: A strategy to integrate evidence-based and patient-centered care
.
Worldviews Evid Based Nurs
.
2006
;
3
(
3
):
116
23
.
[PubMed]
1545-102X
29.
Van Haitsma
K
,
Curyto
K
,
Spector
A
,
Towsley
G
,
Kleban
M
,
Carpenter
B
, et al
The preferences for everyday living inventory: scale development and description of psychosocial preferences responses in community-dwelling elders
.
Gerontologist
.
2013
Aug
;
53
(
4
):
582
95
.
[PubMed]
0016-9013
30.
Boustani
M
,
Watson
L
,
Fultz
B
,
Perkins
AJ
,
Druckenbrod
R
.
Acceptance of dementia screening in continuous care retirement communities: a mailed survey
.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2003
Sep
;
18
(
9
):
780
6
.
[PubMed]
0885-6230
31.
Sullivan
GM
,
Artino
AR
 Jr
.
Analyzing and interpreting data from likert-type scales
.
J Grad Med Educ
.
2013
Dec
;
5
(
4
):
541
2
.
[PubMed]
1949-8349
32.
Boustani
M
,
Perkins
AJ
,
Monahan
P
,
Fox
C
,
Watson
L
,
Hopkins
J
, et al
Measuring primary care patients’ attitudes about dementia screening
.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2008
Aug
;
23
(
8
):
812
20
.
[PubMed]
0885-6230
33.
Loh
PK
,
Ramesh
P
,
Maher
S
,
Saligari
J
,
Flicker
L
,
Goldswain
P
.
Can patients with dementia be assessed at a distance? The use of Telehealth and standardised assessments
.
Intern Med J
.
2004
May
;
34
(
5
):
239
42
.
[PubMed]
1444-0903
34.
Ciemins
EL
,
Holloway
B
,
Jay Coon
P
,
McClosky-Armstrong
T
,
Min
S
: Telemedicine and the Mini-Mental State Examination: Assessment from a Distance. Telemedicine journal and e-health : the official journal of the American Telemedicine Association.
2009
;15(5):476-8.
35.
LaRocque
JR
,
Davis
CL
,
Tan
TP
,
D’Amico
FJ
,
Merenstein
DJ
.
Patient Preferences for Receiving Reports of Test Results
.
J Am Board Fam Med
.
2015
Nov-Dec
;
28
(
6
):
759
66
.
[PubMed]
1557-2625
36.
Hurt
CS
,
Burns
A
,
Brown
RG
,
Barrowclough
C
.
Why don’t older adults with subjective memory complaints seek help?
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2012
Apr
;
27
(
4
):
394
400
.
[PubMed]
1099-1166
37.
Waldorff
FB
,
Rishoj
S
,
Waldemar
G
.
If you don’t ask (about memory), they probably won’t tell
.
J Fam Pract
.
2008
Jan
;
57
(
1
):
41
4
.
[PubMed]
1533-7294
38.
Mitchell
AJ
,
Beaumont
H
,
Ferguson
D
,
Yadegarfar
M
,
Stubbs
B
.
Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand
.
2014
Dec
;
130
(
6
):
439
51
.
[PubMed]
0001-690X
39.
Pinkhasov
RM
,
Wong
J
,
Kashanian
J
,
Lee
M
,
Samadi
DB
,
Pinkhasov
MM
, et al
Are men shortchanged on health? Perspective on health care utilization and health risk behavior in men and women in the United States
.
Int J Clin Pract
.
2010
Mar
;
64
(
4
):
475
87
.
[PubMed]
1368-5031
40.
Galdas
PM
,
Cheater
F
,
Marshall
P
.
Men and health help-seeking behaviour: literature review
.
J Adv Nurs
.
2005
Mar
;
49
(
6
):
616
23
.
[PubMed]
0309-2402
41.
Harada
K
,
Lee
S
,
Shimada
H
,
Lee
S
,
Bae
S
,
Anan
Y
, et al
Psychological predictors of participation in screening for cognitive impairment among community-dwelling older adults
.
Geriatr Gerontol Int
.
2016
.
[PubMed]
1444-1586
42.
Mason
W
,
Suri
S
.
Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk
.
Behav Res Methods
.
2012
Mar
;
44
(
1
):
1
23
.
[PubMed]
1554-351X
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes as well as any distribution of modified material requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.