Background/Aims: Although studies show a negative relationship between physical activity and the risk for cognitive impairment and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease, studies concerning early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EOAD) are lacking. This review aims to justify the value of exercise interventions in EOAD by providing theoretical considerations that include neurobiological processes. Methods: A literature search on key words related to early-onset dementia, exercise, imaging, neurobiological mechanisms, and cognitive reserve was performed. Results/Conclusion: Brain regions and neurobiological processes contributing to the positive effects of exercise are affected in EOAD and, thus, provide theoretical support for exercise interventions in EOAD. Finally, we present the design of a randomized controlled trial currently being conducted in early-onset dementia patients.

Epidemiological studies demonstrate a positive relationship between physical activity and cognitive functioning [1,2]. A decrease in the level of physical activity by a disturbance in gait and, consequently, in walking coincides with a decline in cognitive functioning [1]. A decrease in the level of physical activity might even predict dementia [3]. In contrast, maintaining a physically active lifestyle may protect against dementia [4,5].

A causal relationship, i.e. does physical activity improve cognitive functioning, can only be demonstrated by randomized controlled trials (RCTs). In one study, a daily physical activity intervention in healthy sedentary older persons improved executive functions, in particular working memory [6]. Intervention studies that examine the effects of physical activity on cognition in demented patients are limited and show mixed results. Positive effects on executive functions after aerobic exercise were found in small groups of elderly patients suffering from mild cognitive impairment [7]. Another study also observed positive effects of exercise on cognitive functioning in older persons at risk for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [8]. In contrast, in older persons with moderate dementia, no positive effects on cognition were found, potentially explained by the fact that most of the patients were suffering from concomitant cardiovascular disease [9] (for a review, see [10]). In summary, positive effects of physical activity have been found in healthy older individuals, patients with mild cognitive impairment, and persons at risk for AD. In persons with moderate dementia, these positive effects were not found.

One of the brain regions that play a crucial role in executive functions is the prefrontal cortex [11]. The functions of the prefrontal cortex react positively to increased physical activity [12]. The functioning of other cortical areas, such as the parietal lobe, also show a positive relationship with physical activity [13]. The prefrontal and parietal lobes are particularly vulnerable inearly-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EOAD) [14,15]. It is therefore remarkable that studies examining the effects of physical activity in this population are lacking.

Studies on the effects of physical activity interventions on cognition in this younger population may be worthwhile for a number of reasons. Firstly, few specific treatments are available for patients with EOAD. However, EOAD is increasingly recognised as a problem [16]. It places a large psychological and economic burden on patients and caregivers because of the patient’s prominent role in society at the time of disease onset [17]. A treatment, such as a physical activity program, might bring positive effects for both the patient and the caregiver. Secondly, EOAD patients suffer less from physical inconveniences [18] and may therefore participate in a more intensive program; intensity of an exercise program is important for its effect on cognition [19]. Finally, positive effects of exercise on cognition in normal ageing and (very) early dementia can be explained by their beneficial impact on several neurobiological processes, such as neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, and angiogenesis [20]. Improvement of these processes may also benefit patients with EOAD.

The goal of the present review is to provide theoretical considerations that justify exercise interventions in EOAD. Within this scope, we will address the following topics. First, the physical functioning of EOAD patients will be addressed. Subsequently, brain regions that respond positively to exercise and that are affected in EOAD will be discussed. Next, neurobiological mechanisms such as neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, angiogenesis, and neurotrophins that may underlie the effects of exercise on cognition in EOAD will be highlighted. Subsequently, the cognitive reserve hypothesis will be reviewed. Finally, we present the design of an RCT currently being conducted in early-onset dementia (EOD) patients in our centre.

Search databases were PubMed/MEDLINE and Web of Science. The search terms used were combinations of the key words early-onset and presenile in combination with dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, and frontotemporal dementia. During the literature search, it appeared that studies on EOD involve almost exclusively studies on EOAD. Therefore, this review targets exclusively this type of EOD.

With respect to studies on physical functioning, the following search terms were used: physical functioning, gait (disturbance*), balance, falling, and walking (speed). The search for imaging studies included the following search terms: image*, damage*, atrophy*, degenerate*, single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), (functional) magnetic resonance imaging ((f)MRI), and positron emission tomography (PET), in combination with the terms brain, cortical, cerebral, lobe, cerebrum, encephalon, and grey matter. In relation to exercise, only studies concerning structural MRI were included. Studies on neurogenesis were searched using the terms neuro*, brain, cell, and dendri*, in combination with genesis, growth, branch*, survival, prolifera*, plastic*, death, damage, atrophy*, and degenerat*. The search for studies on synaptogenesis included the term synap*, in combination with genesis, formation, elimination, pruning, synaptophysin, and synaptotagmin. With respect to studies focusing on angiogenesis, the following search terms were used: angio*, arterio*, vessel*, and vascul*, in combination with genesis, growth, branch*, prolifera*, death, deterioration, elimination, and SPECT. Neurotrophin studies were gathered by the search terms neurotrophin*, neurotrophic factor, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), neurotrophin-4 (NT-4), nerve growth factor (NGF), neurotrophin-3 (NT-3), insulin-like growth factor (IGF), and new-neurotrophin-1 (NNT-1). Studies regarding the cognitive reserve hypothesis included the search terms cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and neural reserve. Studies were first selected based on the title. Subsequently, the residual studies were selected using the abstract and the content of the article. The final search was performed in June 2011.

We found one study on gait disturbances in EOAD compared to late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (LOAD) showing that EOAD patients experience less gait disturbances (16% of the patients) than LOAD patients (45%) [18]. In LOAD, gait disturbances have recently been studied (for reviews, see [21,22,23]). Gait disturbances are caused by neuropathology in subcortical brain regions, e.g. basal ganglia, and in cortical areas, e.g. frontal lobe, and can hence be divided in parkinsonian and pseudoparkinsonian gait disturbances, respectively [24]. Gait disturbances can be experienced even in mild stages of AD (i.e. cautious gait) [25,26] and include decreased gait velocity, step length, static and dynamic balance, and a widened base [27]. In more advanced stages of AD, a ‘frontal gait’ can be observed: patients show a shuffling walking style and start and turn difficulties [25].

Taken together, although studies are scarce, we assume that the physical condition of EOAD patients would permit participation in a more intensive exercise program than LOAD patients.

In individuals with higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness, the loss of grey matter, characteristic for ageing, is reduced in prefrontal, superior and anterior parietal, medial temporal (specifically in the hippocampus), and occipital regions [12,28,29,30,31]. It is known that exercise has a beneficial influence on cardiorespiratory fitness [32]. Indeed, after aerobic exercise, increases in grey matter density were observed in prefrontal and temporal cortices; concerning the latter, these increases are particularly seen in the hippocampus [33,34]. For more details see table 1.

Table 1

Brain regions that respond positively to exercise (structural MRI)

Brain regions that respond positively to exercise (structural MRI)
Brain regions that respond positively to exercise (structural MRI)

Neocortical atrophy is a neuropathological hallmark of EOAD [35,36,37]. MRI, SPECT, and PET imaging techniques show that the above-mentioned brain regions which respond positively to exercise are affected in EOAD [14,15,35,36,38,39,40,41,42]. For more details on the aforementioned areas and on areas affected in EOAD but not related to exercise, see table 2.

Table 2

Brain regions that are affected in EOAD

Brain regions that are affected in EOAD
Brain regions that are affected in EOAD

Of note is that the primary sensory and motor areas are relatively preserved in EOAD [35], implying that motor activity is still possible in these patients.

Animal and human experimental studies show that 3 major neurobiological mechanisms underlie the positive effects of exercise on brain structures and subsequently on cognitive function: neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, and angiogenesis [20,43,44].

Neurogenesis

Exercise

In a landmark paper, cell proliferation and neurogenesis have been shown in the hippocampal area of mice having access to a running wheel [45]. Other studies report similar findings, suggesting that prolonged physical activity (voluntary wheel running) enhances neurogenesis in the hippocampus, both in adult mice and rats [46,47,48] as well as in aged mice and rats [49]. More specifically, wheel running in mice stimulates survival of newly generated neurons and their development into functional hippocampal neurons [50]. Furthermore, continued physical activity reduces the adult-dependent decrease in adult neurogenesis [51]. Increased neurogenesis after exercise is mainly coupled with positive effects on BDNF levels, as shown in an early study [52] and supported by several studies (for reviews, see [20,53]). Cerebral blood volume, coupling neuro- and angiogenesis, proves to be elevated in the hippocampus in healthy subjects after 3 months of aerobic exercise [54].

EOAD

Mutations in the presenilin 1 and 2 (PS1 and PS2) genes are linked to most autosomal dominantly inherited forms of EOAD. Several studies suggest an association between PS1 [55,56,57,58] and PS2 mutations [59,60] and cell apoptosis, due to withdrawal of neurotrophins, and amyloid beta disposition [61,62]. PS1 mutations further impair enrichment-induced neurogenesis of hippocampal neural progenitor cells [63]. Although some studies reported increased hippocampal proliferation in EOAD [64,65], this hippocampal proliferation does probably not reflect neurogenesis but rather glial proliferation and vascular changes [64].

Synaptogenesis

Exercise

Animal models show that aerobic training increases synaptic development and synaptic plasticity [66,67]. Voluntary exercise increases dendritic complexity in the dentate gyrus [47,68] and small heat shock proteins and pre- and postsynaptic proteins in the hippocampus in rats [69]. Moderate physical activity changes the level of synaptic proteins in the motor areas of the brain and, hence, may trigger brain plasticity in these areas [70]. Apart from exercise, synaptogenesis has mainly been studied in relation to environmental enrichment. Motor learning (rotorad training) increases synapse formation in the cerebellar cortex of female rats [71,72,73], and acrobatic training enhances synaptogenesis in the motor cortex of male rats [74]. These synaptic changes rely on motor learning and not on the repetitive use of synapses during physical activity only [71]. Cortical levels of synaptophysin are increased after stimulation (living in cages with toys, tunnels, and a running wheel in comparison to regular cages) for 20 weeks [75].

EOAD

Research on synapses is often performed using specific synaptic vesicle proteins, such as synaptophysin and synaptotagmin [76]. The level of synaptophysin is lower in EOAD than in LOAD, indicating a higher synapse loss in EOAD [77]. In a preliminary study, synaptotagmin also seemed to be reduced in both cerebral spinal fluid and brain tissue in EOAD compared to age-matched healthy individuals [78,79]. Greater metabolic dysfunction in the hippocampi and the basal frontal cortex, reflecting greater synapse loss, has been found in EOAD patients carrying the apolipoprotein ε4 (ApoE4) allele, compared to EOAD patients not carrying the ApoE4 allele and LOAD patients [14].

Angiogenesis

Exercise

In rats, prolonged exercise (30 days of wheel running) induces angiogenesis and increased blood flow in the cerebellum, motor cortex, and hippocampus [71,80,81,82]. Angiogenesis occurs in the motor cortex within 30 days from the onset of the exercise program and these effects seem to last over time [82]. Three weeks of exercise reduces neurologic deficits and infarct volume after an induced stroke in rats – a finding that is attributed to angiogenesis [83,84]. Elevated microvessel density is revealed in the striatum after exercise [83,84]. Growth factors that stimulate angiogenesis are already increased after 1–3 weeks of exercise in rats, and the levels of these factors are further elevated after 3 weeks of exercise [84]. Older adults who perform regular exercise show more constant levels of cerebral blood flow in comparison to an inactive control group [85].

EOAD

Angiogenesis is hypothesized to be reduced in AD [86], but it has not been specifically studied in EOAD.

Neurotrophins

Neurotrophins are proteins that support neural networks by stimulating the development of synapses, synaptic efficiency, and survival of neurons [87]. There are several different neurotrophins, e.g. NGF, BDNF, NT-3, NT-4, and IGF-1. Neurotrophins act in brain areas with a high degree of plasticity, such as the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus [88].

Exercise

Several animal experimental studies revealed increased BDNF levels in rats after voluntary exercise [88,89,90,91,92,93,94] in Ammon’s horn areas (CA1 and CA4) of the hippocampus, in layers II and III of the caudal cortex, and in retrosplenial cortices [95]. Also, NGF levels are increased in the dentate gyrus, in CA4 of the hippocampus, and in layers II and III of the caudal cortex after exercise [95]. Both BDNF and NGF levels increased in the motor cortex (layer V; neuron) and in the striatum (glia) after 3 weeks of wheel running [83]. Additionally, IGF-1 levels are increased after exercise [96,97], which is thought to be neuroprotective [98].

EOAD

Mutations in the PS1 and PS2 genes are thought to contribute to apoptotic cell death by means of trophic factor withdrawal [56,62,99]. PS1 mutations may alter cellular signalling systems associated with trophic factor-induced differentiation in PC12 cells [100]. This altered responsiveness to neurotrophic factors could play a role in the pathogenesis of neuritic degeneration and cell death in human PS1 mutation carriers [100]. It is also known that IGF-1 has anti-apoptotic effects. In EOAD, PS1 mutation-related apoptotic neuronal cell death may be caused by disruptions of IGF-1 signalling [101].

In connection with neurogenesis and exercise, the ‘cognitive reserve hypothesis’ has been mentioned. Cognitive reserve is thought to operate as a buffer against cognitive decline in both healthy ageing [102] and neurodegenerative processes [103]. A broad set of determinants contribute to a greater cognitive reserve [104], including exercise [6,105,106]. There is some inconsistency in the use of the term cognitive reserve. The literature shows a classification of cognitive reserve into more passive [107] and more active models [104]. Passive reserve is defined by quantitative neurobiological measures, such as brain volume and the number of neurons and synapses. If more neurons and synaptic connections are present, the brain is able to function longer at a normal level after neuropathological damage resulting from a neurodegenerative process has been inflicted [108]. On the other hand, activereserve implies that the brain actively attempts to cope with neuropathology. Active reserve is determined by how efficient neural networks operate in a healthy brain (neural reserve) and by the ability to compensate via cognitive strategies and the deployment of different neural networks when the pre-existing networks are damaged (neural compensation) [104]. To date, only a few studies have addressed the specific role of cognitive reserve in EOAD. Studies on the relation between passive reserve and EOAD conclude that passive reserve is lower in EOAD than in LOAD, since EOAD patients show clinical symptoms at an earlier age and therefore have a lower premorbid count of neurobiological measures [14,77]. In contrast, studies investigating the association between active reserve and EOAD state that the degree of neuropathology at the moment of symptom onset is greater in EOAD than in LOAD. Active reserve is consequently presumed to be larger in younger than in older patients, since the younger patients are able to cope with a more severe state of neuropathology [109,110].

The notion emerging from this review is that brain regions responding positively to exercise, such as frontal and parietal regions, are particularly affected in EOAD [111]. Damage in these areas results in a variety of EOAD-related clinical features, such as loss of planning skills, loss of initiative, and personality changes [112], which are detrimental to an individual’s autonomy [113]. From this result, it follows that exercise may help fight the symptoms associated with EOAD.

Exercise leads to neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation, to synaptogenesis (particularly) in the cerebellum, to angiogenesis in the motor cortex, and to increased levels of neurotrophins. In EOAD, synapse loss and loss of neurons are neuropathological hallmarks. This suggests that exercise may partly reverse the pathological mechanisms in EOAD.

With respect to the cognitive reserve hypothesis, passive reserve is thought to be lower, and active reserve is considered to be higher in EOAD than in LOAD. Of note is that, to date, only a few studies have addressed this topic directly.

There might be a difference between EOAD and LOAD in the way these disorders respond to a treatment. This difference may be due to a variation in disease progression. Patients with EOAD show a more rapid cognitive decline than patients with LOAD [40,114]. In a meta-analysis, AD patients with a more rapid disease progression showed greater cognitive benefit from rivastigmine treatment than slowly progressing patients [115]. The question arises whether the same may account for nonpharmacological treatments, such as exercise interventions.

This review provides theoretical support for exercise interventions in EOAD. We now present the design of an RCT currently being conducted studying the effects of exercise on the course of dementia in EOD patients.

The EXERcise and Cognition In Sedentary adults with Early-ONset dementia (EXERCISE-ON) study is a multicentre RCT in patients with EOD (AD, vascular dementia, frontotemporal dementia, or other types of dementia). The aim of this study is to assess whether exercise slows down the progressive course of symptoms of EOD. Participants are randomly assigned to 1 of 2 exercise programs: the aerobic exercise program(using a bicycle ergometer) and the flexibility and relaxation program (flexibility and relaxation exercises). Both programs last 3 months, with a frequency of 3 times a week and are situated in a rehabilitation centre. Measurements take place at baseline, after 3 months (end of the exercise program), and after 6 months. Primary outcomes are cognitive functioning (in particular executive functioning), (instrumental) activities of daily living, and quality of life. Secondary measures include physical and neuropsychological measures. Outcome measures will be controlled for comorbid medical conditions (medical chart), depressive symptoms, ApoE genotype, and the rest-activity rhythm in view of possible moderating effects on treatment outcome.

This study is the first to assess the effect of exercise on cognition in EOD patients.

The authors have no financial relationships or conflicts of interest to disclose.

1.
Rosano C, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, Kritchevsky SB, Brach J, Visser M, Yaffe K, Newman AB: Association between physical and cognitive function in healthy elderly: the health, aging and body composition study. Neuroepidemiology 2005;24:8–14.
2.
Verghese J, Wang CL, Lipton RB, Holtzer R, Xue XN: Quantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007;78:929–935.
3.
Waite LM, Grayson DA, Piguet O, Creasey H, Bennett HP, Broe GA: Gait slowing as a predictor of incident dementia: 6-year longitudinal data from the Sydney Older Persons Study. J Neurol Sci 2005;229–230:89–93.
4.
Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B: An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol 2004;3:343–353.
5.
Rovio S, Kåreholt I, Helkala EL, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Kivipelto M: Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol 2005;4:705–711.
6.
Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, Harrison CR, Chason J, Vakil E, Bardell L, Boileau RA, Colcombe A: Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature 1999;400:418–419.
7.
Scherder EJ, Van Paasschen J, Deijen JB, Van Der Knokke S, Orlebeke JFK, Burgers I, Devriese PP, Swaab DF, Sergeant JA: Physical activity and executive functions in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment. Aging Ment Health 2005;9:272–280.
8.
Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, Van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, Greenop KR, Almeida OP: Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease – a randomized trial. JAMA 2008;300:1027–1037.
9.
Eggermont LH, Swaab DF, Hol EM, Scherder EJ: Walking the line: a randomised trial on the effects of a short term walking programme on cognition in dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2009;80:802–804.
10.
Eggermont L, Swaab D, Luiten P, Scherder E: Exercise, cognition and Alzheimer’s disease: more is not necessarily better. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:562–575.
11.
Stuss DT, Alexander MP: Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol Res 2000;63:289–298.
12.
Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Raz N, Webb AG, Cohen NJ, McAuley E, Kramer AF: Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2003;58:176–180.
13.
Rosano C, Venkatraman VK, Guralnik J, Newman AB, Glynn NW, Launer L, Taylor CA, Williamson J, Studenski S, Pahor M, Aizenstein H: Psychomotor speed and functional brain MRI 2 years after completing a physical activity treatment. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2010;65:639–647.
14.
Mosconi L, Herholz K, Prohovnik I, Nacmias B, De Cristofaro MT, Fayyaz M, Bracco L, Sorbi S, Pupi A: Metabolic interaction between ApoE genotype and onset age in Alzheimer’s disease: implications for brain reserve. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:15–23.
15.
Seo SW, Im K, Lee JM, Kim ST, Ahn HJ, Go SM, Kim SH, Na DL: Effects of demographic factors on cortical thickness in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2011;32:200–209.
16.
Sampson EL, Warren JD, Rossor MN: Young onset dementia. Postgrad Med J 2004;80:125–139.
17.
Shinagawa S, Ikeda M, Toyota Y, Matsumoto T, Matsumoto N, Mori T, Ishikawa T, Fukuhara R, Komori K, Hokoishi K, Tanabe H: Frequency and clinical characteristics of early-onset dementia in consecutive patients in a memory clinic. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2007;24:42–47.
18.
Seltzer B, Sherwin I: A comparison of clinical-features in early-onset and late-onset primary degenerative dementia – one entity or 2. Arch Neurol 1983;40:143–146.
19.
Rolland Y, van Kan GA, Vellas B: Physical activity and Alzheimer’s disease: from prevention to therapeutic perspectives. J Am Med Dir Assoc 2008;9:390–405.
20.
Churchill JD, Galvez R, Colcombe S, Swain RA, Kramer AF, Greenough WT: Exercise, experience and the aging brain. Neurobiol Aging 2002;23:941–955.
21.
Scherder E, Eggermont L, Sergeant J, Boersma F: Physical activity and cognition in Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to vascular risk factors, executive functions and gait. Rev Neurosci 2007;18:149–158.
22.
Scherder E, Eggermont L, Visscher C, Scheltens P, Swaab D: Understanding higher level gait disturbances in mild dementia in order to improve rehabilitation: ‘last in-first out’. Neurosci Biobehav Rev2011;35:699–714.
23.
Van Iersel MB, Hoefsloot W, Munneke M, Bloem BR, Olde Rikkert MG: Systematic review of quantitative clinical gait analysis in patients with dementia. Z Gerontol Geriatr 2004;37:27–32.
24.
Kurlan RE, Richard IH, Papka M, Marshall F: Movement disorders in Alzheimer’s disease: more rigidity of definitions is needed. Mov Disord 2000;15:24–29.
25.
O’Keeffe ST, Kazeem H, Philpott RM, Playfer JR, Gosney M, Lye M: Gait disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical study. Age Ageing 1996;25:313–316.
26.
Prehogan A, Cohen CI: Motor dysfunction in dementias. Geriatrics 2004;59:53–60.
27.
Pettersson AF, Engardt M, Wahlund LO: Activity level and balance in subjects with mild Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2002;13:213–216.
28.
Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Chaddock L, Hu L, Morris KS, White SM, Wójcicki TR, McAuley E, Kramer AF: Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 2009;19:1030–1039.
29.
Erickson KI, Raji CA, Lopez OL, Becker JT, Rosano C, Newman AB, Gach HM, Thompson PM, Ho AJ, Kuller LH: Physical activity predicts gray matter volume in late adulthood: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Neurology 2010;75:1415–1422.
30.
Gordon BA, Rykhlevskaia EI, Brumback CR, Lee Y, Elavsky S, Konopack JF, McAuley E, Kramer AF, Colcombe S, Gratton G, Fabiani M: Neuroanatomical correlates of aging, cardiopulmonary fitness level, and education. Psychophysiology 2008;45:825–838.
31.
Rovio S, Spulber G, Nieminen LJ, Niskanen E, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Nissinen A, Soininen H, Kivipelto M: The effect of midlife physical activity on structural brain changes in the elderly. Neurobiol Aging 2010;31:1927–1936.
32.
American College of Sports Medicine Position Stand. The recommended quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness, and flexibility in healthy adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1998;30:975–991.
33.
Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, Elavsky S, Marquez DX, Hu L, Kramer AF: Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;61:1166–1170.
34.
Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey E, Vieira VJ, Martin SA, Pence BD, Woods JA, McAuley E, Kramer AF: Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011;108:3017–3022.
35.
Frisoni GB, Pievani M, Testa C, Sabattoli F, Bresciani L, Bonetti M, Beltramello A, Hayashi KM, Toga AW, Thompson PW: The topography of grey matter involvement in early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2007;130:720–730.
36.
Ishii K, Kawachi T, Sasaki H, Kono AK, Fukuda T, Kojima Y, Mori E: Voxel-based morphometric comparison between early- and late-onset mild Alzheimer’s disease and assessment of diagnostic performance of Z score images. Am J Neuroradiol 2005;26:333–340.
37.
Mizuno Y, Ikeda K, Tsuchiya K, Ishihara R, Shibayama H: Two distinct subgroups of senile dementia of Alzheimer type: quantitative study of neurofibrillary tangles. Neuropathology 2003;23:282–289.
38.
Johnson KA, Lopera F, Jones K, Becker A, Sperling R, Hilson J, Londono J, Siegert I, Arcos M, Moreno S, Madrigal L, Ossa J, Pineda N, Ardila A, Roselli M, Albert MS, Kosik KS, Rios A: Presenilin-1-associated abnormalities in regional cerebral perfusion. Neurology 2001;56:1545–1551.
39.
Kemp PM, Holmes C, Hoffmann SMA, Bolt L, Holmes R, Rowden J, Fleming JS: Alzheimer’s disease: differences in technetium-99m HMPAO SPECT scan findings between early-onset and late onset dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003;74:715–719.
40.
Rabinovici GD, Furst AJ, Alkalay A, Racine CA, O’Neil JP, Janabi M, Baker SL, Agarwal N, Bonasera SJ, Mormino EC, Weiner MW, Gorno-Tempini ML, Rosen HJ, Miller BL, Jagust WJ: Increased metabolic vulnerability in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease is not related to amyloid burden. Brain 2010;133:512–528.
41.
Shiino A, Watanabe T, Maeda K, Kotani E, Akiguchi I, Matsuda M: Four subgroups of Alzheimer’s disease based on patterns of atrophy using VBM and a unique pattern for early-onset disease. Neuroimage 2006;33:17–26.
42.
Shiino A, Watanabe T, Kitagawa T, Kotani E, Takahashi J, Morikawa S, Akiguchi I: Different atrophic patterns in early- and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease and evaluation of clinical utility of a method of regional z-score analysis using voxel-based morphometry. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008;26:175–186.
43.
Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Colcombe SJ: Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain. J Appl Physiol 2006;101:1237–1242.
44.
Mcauley E, Kramer AF, Colcombe SJ: Cardiovascular fitness and neurocognitive function in older adults: a brief review. Brain Behav Immun 2004;18:214–220.
45.
van Praag H, Christie BR, Sejnowski TJ, Gage FH: Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999;96:13427–13431.
46.
Browne L, Sweeney BJ, Farrell MA: Late-onset neuroaxonal leucoencephalopathy with spheroids and vascular amyloid. Eur Neurol 2003;50:85–90.
47.
Redila VA, Christie BR: Exercise-induced changes in dendritic structure and complexity in the adult hippocampal dentate gyrus. Neuroscience 2006;137:1299–1307.
48.
Farmer J, Zhao X, van Praag H, Wodtke K, Gage FH, Christie BR: Effects of voluntary exercise on synaptic plasticity and gene expression in the dentate gyrus of adult male Sprague-Dawley rats in vivo. Neuroscience 2004;124:71–79.
49.
van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao CM, Gage FH: Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neurosci 2005;25:8680–8685.
50.
Bednarczyk MR, Aumont A, Decary S, Bergeron R, Fernandes KJL: Prolonged voluntary wheel-running stimulates neural precursors in the hippocampus and forebrain of adult CD1 mice. Hippocampus 2009;19:913–927.
51.
Kronenberg G, Bick-Sander A, Bunk E, Wolf C, Ehninger D, Kempermann G: Physical exercise prevents age-related decline in precursor cell activity in the mouse dentate gyrus. Neurobiol Aging 2006;27:1505–1513.
52.
Neeper SA, Gómez-Pinilla F, Choi J, Cotman C: Exercise and brain neurotrophins. Nature 1995;373:109.
53.
Cotman CW, Berchtold NC: Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci 2002;25:295–301.
54.
Pereira AC, Huddleston DE, Brickman AM, Sosunov AA, Hen R, McKhann GM, Sloan R, Gage FH, Brown TR, Small SA: An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007;104:5638–5643.
55.
Guo Q, Furukawa K, Sopher BL, Pham DG, Xie J, Robinson N, Martin GM, Mattson MP: Alzheimer’s PS-1 mutation perturbs calcium homeostasis and sensitizes PC12 cells to death induced by amyloid beta-peptide. Neuroreport 1996;8:379–383.
56.
Guo Q, Sopher BL, Furukawa K, Pham DG, Robinson N, Martin GM, Mattson MP: Alzheimer’s presenilin mutation sensitizes neural cells to apoptosis induced by trophic factor withdrawal and amyloid beta-peptide: involvement of calcium and oxyradicals. J Neurosci 1997;17:4212–4222.
57.
Gupta S, Singh R, Datta P, Zhang Z, Orr C, Lu Z, DuBois G, Zervos AS, Meisler MH, Srinivasula SM, Fernandes-Alnemri T, Alnemri ES: The C-terminal tail of presenilin regulates Omi/HtrA2 protease activity. J Biol Chem 2004;279:45844–45854.
58.
Morgan GA, Guo Q, Chan SL, Gary DS, Osborne BA, Mattson MP: Defects of immune regulation in the presenilin-1 mutant knockin mouse. Neuromolecular Med 2007;9:35–45.
59.
da Costa CA, Paitel E, Mattson MP, Amson R, Telerman A, Ancolio K, Checler F: Wild-type and mutated presenilins 2 trigger p53-dependent apoptosis and down-regulate presenilin 1 expression in HEK293 human cells and in murine neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002;99:4043–4048.
60.
Janicki S, Monteiro MJ: Increased apoptosis arising from increased expression of the Alzheimer’s disease-associated presenilin-2 mutation (N141I). J Cell Biol 1997;139:485–495.
61.
Mattson MP, Robinson N, Guo Q: Estrogens stabilize mitochondrial function and protect neural cells against the pro-apoptotic action of mutant presenilin-1. Neuroreport 1997;8:3817–3821.
62.
Mattson MP, Guo Q, Furukawa K, Pedersen WA: Presenilins, the endoplasmic reticulum, and neuronal apoptosis in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurochem 1998;70:1–14.
63.
Choi SH, Veeraraghavalu K, Lazarov O, Marler S, Ransohoff RM, Ramirez JM, Sisodia SS: Non-cell-autonomous effects of presenilin 1 variants on enrichment-mediated hippocampal progenitor cell proliferation and differentiation. Neuron 2008;59:568–580.
64.
Boekhoorn K, Joels M, Lucassen PJ: Increased proliferation reflects glial and vascular-associated changes, but not neurogenesis in the presenile Alzheimer hippocampus. Neurobiol Dis 2006;24:1–14.
65.
Ramirez-Rodriguez G, Itez-King G, Kempermann G: The new neuron formation in the adult hippocampus: neurogenesis. Salud Ment 2007;30:12–19.
66.
Lu B, Chow A: Neurotrophins and hippocampal synaptic transmission and plasticity. J Neurosci Res 1999;58:76–87.
67.
Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F: Interplay between brain-derived neurotrophic factor and signal transduction modulators in the regulation of the effects of exercise on synaptic-plasticity. Neuroscience 2003;122:647–657.
68.
Eadie BD, Redila VA, Christie BR: Voluntary exercise alters the cytoarchitecture of the adult dentate gyrus by increasing cellular proliferation, dendritic complexity, and spine density. J Comp Neurol 2005;486:39–47.
69.
Hu SX, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F, Frautschy SA: Exercise can increase small heat shock proteins (sHSP) and pre- and post-synaptic proteins in the hippocampus. Brain Res 2009;1249:191–201.
70.
Ferreira AFB, Real CC, Rodrigues AC, Alves AS, Britto LRG: Moderate exercise changes synaptic and cytoskeletal proteins in motor regions of the rat brain. Brain Res 2010;1361:31–42.
71.
Black JE, Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara AA, Greenough WT: Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1990;87:5568–5572.
72.
Kleim JA, Vij K, Ballard DH, Greenough WT: Learning-dependent synaptic modifications in the cerebellar cortex of the adult rat persist for at least four weeks. J Neurosci 1997;17:717–721.
73.
Kleim JA, Swain RA, Armstrong KA, Napper RMA, Jones TA, Greenough WT: Selective synaptic plasticity within the cerebellar cortex following complex motor skill learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1998;69:274–289.
74.
Jones TA, Chu CJ, Grande LA, Gregory AD: Motor skills training enhances lesion-induced structural plasticity in the motor cortex of adult rats. J Neurosci 1999;19:10153–10163.
75.
Levi O, Jongen-Relo AL, Feldon J, Roses AD, Michaelson DM: ApoE4 impairs hippocampal plasticity isoform-specifically and blocks the environmental stimulation of synaptogenesis and memory. Neurobiol Dis 2003;13:273–282.
76.
Thome J, Pesold B, Baader M, Hu M, Gewirtz JC, Duman RS, Henn FA: Stress differentially regulates synaptophysin and synaptotagmin expression in hippocampus. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50:809–812.
77.
Bigio EH, Hynan LS, Sontag E, Satumtira S, White CL: Synapse loss is greater in presenile than senile onset Alzheimer disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol2002;28:218–227.
78.
Davidsson P, Jahn R, Bergquist J, Ekman R, Blennow K: Synaptotagmin, a synaptic vesicle protein, is present in human cerebrospinal fluid: a new biochemical marker for synaptic pathology in Alzheimer disease? Mol Chem Neuropathol 1996;27:195–210.
79.
Gottfries CG, Lehmann W, Regland B: Early diagnosis of cognitive impairment in the elderly with the focus on Alzheimer’s disease. J Neural Transm 1998;105:773–786.
80.
Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara AA, Black JE, Greenough WT: Exercise and the brain: angiogenesis in the adult rat cerebellum after vigorous physical activity and motor skill learning. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1992;12:110–119.
81.
Kleim JA, Cooper NR, Van den Berg PA: Exercise induces angiogenesis but does not alter movement representations within rat motor cortex. Brain Res 2002;934:1–6.
82.
Swain RA, Harris AB, Wiener EC, Dutka MV, Morris HD, Theien BE, Konda S, Engberg K, Lauterbur PC, Greenough WT: Prolonged exercise induces angiogenesis and increases cerebral blood volume in primary motor cortex of the rat. Neuroscience 2003;117:1037–1046.
83.
Ding Y, Li J, Luan X, Ding YH, Lai Q, Rafols JA, Phillis JW, Clark JC, Diaz FG: Exercise pre-conditioning reduces brain damage in ischemic rats that may be associated with regional angiogenesis and cellular overexpression of neurotrophin. Neuroscience 2004;124:583–591.
84.
Ding YH, Luan XD, Li J, Rafols JA, Guthinkonda M, Diaz FG, Ding Y: Exercise-induced overexpression of angiogenic factors and reduction of ischemia/reperfusion injury in stroke. Curr Neurovasc Res 2004;1:411–420.
85.
Rogers RL, Meyer JS, Mortel KF: After reaching retirement age physical-activity sustains cerebral perfusion and cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990;38:123–128.
86.
Vagnucci AH, Li WW: Alzheimer’s disease and angiogenesis. Lancet 2003;361:605–608.
87.
Meisner F, Scheller C, Kneitz S, Sopper S, Neuen-Jacob E, Riederer P, Ter Meulen V, Koutsilieri E: Memantine upregulates BDNF and prevents dopamine deficits in SIV-infected macaques: a novel pharmacological action of memantine. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33:2228–2236.
88.
Tapia-Arancibia L, Aliaga E, Silhol M, Arancibia S: New insights into brain BDNF function in normal aging and Alzheimer disease. Brain Res Rev 2008;59:201–220.
89.
Cotman CW, Berchtold NC: Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci 2002;25:295–301.
90.
Farmer J, Zhao X, van Praag H, Wodtke K, Gage FH, Christie BR: Effects of voluntary exercise on synaptic plasticity and gene expression in the dentate gyrus of adult male Sprague-Dawley rats in vivo. Neuroscience 2004;124:71–79.
91.
Gómez-Pinilla F, Ying Z, Roy RR, Molteni R, Edgerton VR: Voluntary exercise induces a BDNF-mediated mechanism that promotes neuroplasticity. J Neurophysiol 2002;88:2187–2195.
92.
Oliff HS, Berchtold NC, Isackson P, Cotman CW: Exercise-induced regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) transcripts in the rat hippocampus. Mol Brain Res 1998;61:147–153.
93.
Seifert T, Brassard P, Wissenberg M, Rasmussen P, Nordby P, Stallknecht B, Adser H, Jakobsen AH, Pilegaard H, Nielsen HB, Secher NH: Endurance training enhances BDNF release from the human brain. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol2010;298:372–377.
94.
Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F: Exercise induces BDNF and synapsin I to specific hippocampal subfields. J Neurosci Res 2004;76:356–362.
95.
Neeper SA, Gomezpinilla F, Choi J, Cotman CW: Physical activity increases mRNA for brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor in rat brain. Brain Res 1996;726:49–56.
96.
Llorens-Martin M, Torres-Aleman I, Trejo JL: Mechanisms mediating brain plasticity: IGF1 and adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Neuroscientist 2009;15:134–148.
97.
Trejo JL, Carro E, Torres-Aleman I: Circulating insulin-like growth factor I mediates exercise-induced increases in the number of new neurons in the adult hippocampus. J Neurosci 2001;21:1628–1634.
98.
Carro E, Trejo JL, Busiguina S, Torres-Aleman I: Circulating insulin-like growth factor I mediates the protective effects of physical exercise against brain insults of different etiology and anatomy. J Neurosci 2001;21:5678–5684.
99.
Mattson MP, Guo Q: Cell and molecular neurobiology of presenilins: a role for the endoplasmic reticulum in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease? J Neurosci Res 1997;50:505–513.
100.
Furukawa KS, Guo Q, Schellenberg GD, Mattson MP: Presenilin-1 mutation alters NGF-induced neurite outgrowth, calcium homeostasis, and transcription factor (AP-1) activation in PC12 cells. J Neurosci Res 1998;52:618–624.
101.
Tanii H, Ankarcrona M, Flood F, Nilsberth C, Mehta ND, Perez-Tur J, Winblad B, Benedikz E, Cowburn RF: Alzheimer’s disease presenilin-1 exon 9 deletion and L250S mutations sensitize SH-SY5Y neuroblastoma cells to hyperosmotic stress-induced apoptosis. Neuroscience 2000;95:593–601.
102.
Corral M, Rodriguez M, Amenedo E, Sanchez JL, Diaz F: Cognitive reserve, age, and neuropsychological performance in healthy participants. Dev Neuropsychol 2006;29:479–491.
103.
Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P: Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review. Psychol Med 2006;36:441–454.
104.
Stern Y: Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:2015–2028.
105.
Lam LC, Tam CW, Lui VW, Chan WC, Chan SS, Chiu HF, Wong A, Tham MK, Ho KS, Chan WM: Modality of physical exercise and cognitive function in Hong Kong older Chinese community. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;24:48–53.
106.
Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth MEJ: Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Soc Sci Med 2003;56:785–792.
107.
Katzman R: Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1993;43:13–20.
108.
Mori E, Hirono N, Yamashita H, Imamura T, Ikejiri Y, Ikeda M, Kitagaki H, Shimomura T, Yoneda Y: Premorbid brain size as a determinant of reserve capacity against intellectual decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154:18–24.
109.
Kim EJ, Cho SS, Jeong Y, Park KC, Kang SJ, Kang E, Kim SE, Lee KH, Na DL: Glucose metabolism in early onset versus late onset Alzheimer’s disease: an SPM analysis of 120 patients. Brain 2005;128:1790–1801.
110.
Marshall GA, Fairbanks LA, Tekin S, Vinters HV, Cummings JL: Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease is associated with greater pathologic burden. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2007;20:29–33.
111.
McMurtray A, Clark DG, Christine D, Mendez MF: Early-onset dementia: frequency and causes compared to late-onset dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:59–64.
112.
Kelley BJ, Boeve BF, Josephs KA: Cognitive and noncognitive neurological features of young-onset dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;27:564–571.
113.
Cahn-Weiner DA, Farias ST, Julian L, Harvey DJ, Kramer JH, Reed BR, Mungas D, Wetzel M, Chui H: Cognitive and neuroimaging predictors of instrumental activities of daily living. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13:747–757.
114.
Van der Vlies AE, Koedam EL, Pijnenburg YA, Twisk JW, Scheltens P, van der Flier WM: Most rapid cognitive decline in APOE epsilon 4 negative Alzheimer’s disease with early onset. Psychol Med 2009;39:1907–1911.
115.
Farlow MR, Small GW, Quarg P, Krause A: Efficacy of rivastigmine in Alzheimer’s disease patients with rapid disease progression: results of a meta-analysis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2005;20:192–197.
116.
Frisoni GB, Testa C, Sabattoli F, Beltramello A, Soininen H, Laakso MP: Structural correlates of early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease: voxel based morphometric study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:112–114.
117.
Karas G, Scheltens P, Rombouts S, Van Schijndel R, Kleim M, Jones B, van der Flier W, Vrenken H, Barkhof F: Precuneus atrophy in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease: a morphometric structural MRI study. Neuroradiology 2007;49:967–976.
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Open Access License: This is an Open Access article licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 3.0 Unported license (CC BY-NC) (www.karger.com/OA-license), applicable to the online version of the article only. Distribution permitted for non-commercial purposes only.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.