Background: Cerebral small vessel disease (SVD) is a leading cause of cognitive decline in the elderly. Cerebral microbleeds (CMBs) have emerged as an important manifestation of cerebral SVD, in addition to lacunar infarcts and white matter lesions (WMLs). We investigated whether the presence and location of CMBs in elderly subjects were associated with cognitive function, independent of lacunar infarcts and WMLs. Methods: One hundred and forty-eight nondemented elderly with SVD, defined as the presence of lacunar infarcts and/or WMLs on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), were studied. Executive function and global cognition were assessed by the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), respectively. The differences in the scores for the FAB and MMSE between CMB-positive and CMB-negative subjects were calculated after adjusting for possible confounders. Results: The mean age of the subjects was 72.4 w 8.6 years. CMBs were detected in 48 subjects (32%), with a mean number of CMBs per subject of 1.6 (range 0-31). Among CMB-positive subjects, 42 (87.5%) had CMBs in deep or infratentorial regions with or without lobar CMBs, and 6 (12.5%) had CMBs in strictly lobar regions. The presence of CMBs was significantly associated with FAB and MMSE scores after adjustment for age, years of education, brain volume and the presence of lacunar infarcts (for the FAB) or severe WMLs (for the MMSE). The presence of CMBs in the basal ganglia, in the thalamus or in the lobar regions was associated with FAB scores, while that in the lobar regions was associated with MMSE scores. However, there was no association between CMBs in the infratentorial regions and cognitive parameters. Conclusions: In nondemented elderly with SVD on MRI, the presence of CMBs was independently associated with worse executive and global cognitive functions. CMBs seemed to reflect hypertensive microangiopathy in this population, and CMBs in specific areas may play an important role in cognitive function. i 2014 S. Karger AG, Basel

Cerebral small vessel disease (SVD) is a leading cause of cognitive decline in the elderly [1]. Neuroimaging features of SVD, including lacunar infarcts and white matter lesions (WMLs), are frequently found by brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of elderly people, and are associated with cognitive dysfunction [2,3,4].

Recently, cerebral microbleeds (CMBs) have emerged as an important new manifestation of SVD that can be detected by gradient-echo T2*-weighted MRI [5]. Histopathological examinations showed that CMBs are focal hemosiderin depositions in the perivascular space [6,7]. CMBs in deep or infratentorial regions may reflect hypertensive or atherosclerotic microangiopathy, while CMBs in lobar regions may reflect cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA) [8,9,10,11]. There has been increasing evidence suggesting an association between CMBs and cognitive function [12]. For example, the presence of CMBs has been reported to be associated with cognitive impairment in healthy adults [13,14], general elderly populations [15,16], elderly with increased vascular risk [17] and stroke patients [18,19,20,21]. Because CMBs are frequently identified in association with other MRI evidence of SVD, such as WMLs and lacunar infarcts [22], CMBs could be an important cause of cognitive decline in elderly subjects with SVD. However, few studies have so far examined the effect of CMBs on cognition in such a population.

In the present study, we examined nondemented elderly with SVD, defined as the presence of lacunar infarcts and/or WMLs on MRI, and investigated whether the presence and location of CMBs were independently associated with cognitive function.

Patients

A total of 148 nondemented elderly with SVD were examined in the present study. The subjects were enrolled from the outpatients who underwent brain MRI in the Department of Neurology, Juntendo University Shizuoka Hospital, from December 2010 to February 2012. Brain MRI was performed for the assessment of cerebrovascular diseases on the outpatients with ≥1 vascular risk factors, or with a history of stroke or transient ischemic attack, or with symptoms suspected to be due to cerebrovascular diseases such as dizziness, dysarthria, numbness or weakness of limbs or gait disturbance. The inclusion criteria were the presence of SVD, such as lacunar infarcts and/or WMLs defined as at least Fazekas grade 1 on MRI [23]. The exclusion criteria were territorial infarcts, cerebral hemorrhage, dementia including Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia, recent/current use of acetylcholine esterase inhibitors, neurodegenerative diseases, psychiatric disease or white matter disease other than ischemia. Dementia was diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, ed. 4 [24]. Patients with a disability that affected cognitive assessment and those with a disability within 6 months after symptomatic stroke were also excluded.

All study participants provided informed consent, and the study design was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the hospital.

Risk Factors

Cardiovascular risk factors were examined during the study visit by interviews and laboratory examinations. Hypertension was defined as having a systolic blood pressure ≥140 mm Hg and/or a diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mm Hg and/or taking antihypertensive medication. Diabetes mellitus was defined as having a fasting blood glucose level ≥126 mg/dl and/or a HbA1c level ≥6.5% and/or using insulin or oral hypoglycemic agents. Dyslipidemia was defined as having a total cholesterol level ≥220 mg/dl and/or a triglyceride level ≥150 mg/dl and/or using lipid-lowering medication. The subjects were considered to be current smokers if they had smoked at least 1 cigarette a day within the previous year. The length of education and history of symptomatic stroke were assessed by self-reports.

Brain MRI

Brain MRI was performed using a 1.5-tesla MR system (Siemens, Magnetom Avanto), and the whole brain was scanned at a slice thickness of 6 mm and an interslice gap of 0.6 mm, obtaining 19 axial images. The imaging protocol consisted of axial T2*-weighted gradient echo [repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE) = 685/25 ms, flip angle = 20°, field of view (FOV) = 207 × 230, matrix = 218 × 256], axial T1-weighted spin echo (TR/TE = 500/13 ms, FOV = 207 × 230, matrix = 205 × 256), axial T2-weighted fast spin echo (TR/TE = 3,800/ 91 ms, FOV = 207 × 230, matrix = 288 × 384) and axial fluid-attenuated inversion recovery imaging (FLAIR; TR/TE = 8,500/79 ms, inversion time = 2,500 ms, FOV = 207 × 230, matrix = 230 × 256).

CMBs were defined as small, homogeneous, round foci of low signal intensities on T2*-weighted images that were <10 mm in diameter. Basal ganglia calcification and vascular flow voids were excluded. The Microbleed Anatomical Rating Scale (MARS) [25] was used to guide the identification and to describe the location of CMBs. A lacunar infarct was defined as a small lesion (3-15 mm in diameter) with a high signal intensity on T2-weighted images and a low signal intensity on T1-weighted and FLAIR images. Deep and periventricular WMLs were graded from 0 to 3 according to the Fazekas scale [23]. Periventricular hyperintensity or deep white matter hyperintensity of grade ≥2 was defined as a severe WML [14]. The brain volume (%brain) as an index of brain atrophy was calculated by using the methods described by Koga et al. [26]. Briefly, the area of the cerebral parenchyma on T2-weighted images at two slices above the level of the pineal body was quantitatively measured using a computer-assisted processing system (Image J version 1.48; National Institutes of Health) and was divided by the area inside the skull at the same level to correct for individual differences in head size. The images were analyzed by a trained observer blinded to the patients' clinical data.

Cognitive Function

The cognitive assessment was administered within 2 months of the MRI. Executive function was tested using the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) [27]. The FAB is a bedside cognitive and behavioral battery used to assess executive function which consists of 6 subsets: similarities (conceptualization), lexical fluency (mental flexibility), motor series (programming), conflicting instructions (sensitivity to interference), go/no go (inhibitory control) and prehension behavior (environmental autonomy). Global cognition was also tested using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [28].

Statistical Analysis

The statistical analyses were performed with the JMP version 9.0 software program (SAS Inc., Cary, N.C., USA). A value of p < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. To analyze the associations between the cognitive battery scores and participants' characteristics, we used the Pearson correlation coefficient for continuous variables and the two-sample t test for categorical variables. The scores for the total and subtests of the FAB and MMSE were compared between CMB-negative and CMB-positive subjects adjusted for age, sex and covariates that were significantly associated with the cognitive battery scores. The relationship between the number of CMBs and the cognitive battery scores was also investigated using a multiple linear regression analysis adjusted for the same covariates.

The characteristics of the subjects are shown in table 1. The mean age of the study participants was 72.4 ± 8.6 years, and 53% were male. A total of 238 CMBs were found in 48 subjects (32%), with a mean number of CMBs per subject of 1.6 (range 0-31). Of these, 18 subjects (37.5%) had 1 CMB, 12 (25%) had 2-4 CMBs, 12 (25%) had 5-9 CMBs, and 6 (12.5%) had ≥10 CMBs. Among the CMB-positive subjects, 42 (87.5%) had CMBs in deep or infratentorial regions with or without lobar CMBs, and 6 (12.5%) had CMBs in strictly lobar regions. Thirteen subjects (31.0%) had CMBs in both deep and lobar regions. Twenty-five subjects (59.5%) had CMBs in the basal ganglia, 25 (59.5%) had CMBs in the thalamus, 20 (47.6%) had CMBs in the infratentorial regions, and 19 (45.2%) had CMBs in lobar regions (10 in the frontal lobe, 2 in the parietal lobe, 8 in the temporal lobe and 9 in the occipital lobe). The mean age was significantly lower in subjects with CMBs than in those without (70.0 ± 8.0 vs. 73.5 ± 8.6 years, p < 0.05). The prevalence of hypertension was significantly higher in subjects with CMBs than in those without (89.6 vs 70.0%, p < 0.01), while the rates of other vascular risk factors, such as diabetes mellitus, dyslipidemia and current smoking, were not significantly different between the groups.

Table 1

The characteristics of the study subjects (n = 148)

The characteristics of the study subjects (n = 148)
The characteristics of the study subjects (n = 148)

The mean FAB total score was 14.7 ± 2.6 (range 8-18), and the mean MMSE total score was 27.6 ± 2.3 (range 22-30). The associations between the cognitive battery scores and subject characteristics are shown in table 2. The FAB total score was significantly correlated with age (r = -0.3359), years of education (r = 0.3676) and %brain (r = 0.3858) and was significantly different between subjects with and without CMBs as well as between those with and without lacunar infarcts. The MMSE total score was significantly correlated with the subjects' age (r = -0.1803), years of education (r = 0.2437), number of CMBs (r = -0.2081) and %brain (r = 0.2844) and was significantly different between subjects with and without CMBs as well as between those with and without severe WMLs.

Table 2

The associations of FAB and MMSE total scores with the subject characteristics

The associations of FAB and MMSE total scores with the subject characteristics
The associations of FAB and MMSE total scores with the subject characteristics

Table 3 shows a comparison of the cognitive parameters between CMB-negative and CMB-positive subjects. The CMB-positive subjects had significantly lower scores on the FAB (total score, similarities, go/no go) and the MMSE (total score, attention and calculation, repetition) than did the CMB-negative subjects after adjustment for age, sex, years of education, %brain and the presence of lacunar infarcts (for the FAB) or severe WMLs (for the MMSE). The number of CMBs was not significantly associated with MMSE scores after adjustment for the same covariates.

Table 3

The results of a comparison of the cognitive parameters between CMB-positive and CMB-negative subjects

The results of a comparison of the cognitive parameters between CMB-positive and CMB-negative subjects
The results of a comparison of the cognitive parameters between CMB-positive and CMB-negative subjects

The association between the locations of CMBs and cognitive parameters was also analyzed after adjusting for the same covariates (table 4). The presence of CMBs in the basal ganglia, in the thalamus or in the lobar regions was associated with a significant reduction in FAB total score. Subjects with lobar CMBs had significantly lower scores on the MMSE (total, attention and calculation, repetition) than those without. In addition, the subjects with thalamic CMBs had significantly lower scores on the ‘orientation' subtests of the MMSE. There was no association between the presence of CMBs in infratentorial regions and cognitive parameters.

Table 4

The relationship between locations of CMBs and cognitive parameters

The relationship between locations of CMBs and cognitive parameters
The relationship between locations of CMBs and cognitive parameters

Our study showed that the presence of CMBs was significantly associated with both executive function and global cognition in nondemented elderly individuals with SVD, which was defined as the presence of lacunar infarcts and/or WMLs on MRI. This association was independent of coexisting lacunar infarcts, WMLs and other possible confounders.

An association between CMBs and global cognitive dysfunction, as assessed by the MMSE or Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), has been reported in the healthy adults [14,15], a general elderly population [16] and stroke patients [21]. We used the MMSE to assess cognition, because this scale is a widely used instrument of cognitive assessment for patients with SVD. However, it has been suggested that the MMSE has a relatively low sensitivity for the assessment of cognitive function in these patients [28]. Executive dysfunction is a characteristic feature of SVD [29], and CMBs have been reported to be associated with executive function in the general elderly populations [15,16] and stroke patients [18,19,20]. In our study, we used the FAB to assess executive function. The FAB score has been reported to be associated with the severity of WMLs in atherosclerotic high-risk patients without dementia [30]. Our study showed that executive function and global cognition, as assessed by the FAB and MMSE, were significantly associated with the presence of CMBs in nondemented elderly individuals with SVD. The RUN DMC Study has also examined nondemented elderly individuals with SVD on MRI [31]. The study group showed that the presence of CMBs was associated with impairment of global cognition and executive function, such as psychomotor speed and attention, which is consistent with the results of our study.

In our study, when the presence of CMBs was divided into specific locations, executive function was found to be associated with CMBs in the basal ganglia as well as in the thalamus and in the lobar regions, while global cognition was associated with CMBs in the lobar regions. However, there was no association between CMBs in the infratentorial regions and the cognitive parameters. These findings suggest that CMBs in specific areas could play a significant role in cognitive function. The frontal lobe, the basal ganglia and the thalamus participate in the frontal-subcortical circuits involved in executive function [32]. The presence of CMBs in these regions has been reported to be associated with cognitive impairment in a general elderly population [14], stroke patients [18,19,20,21] and nondemented elderly individuals with SVD in the RUN DMC Study [31]. It has been suggested that CMBs may cause direct structural damage to the surrounding brain tissue, leading to the disconnection of functionally important cortical and subcortical structures [18]. In addition, the presence of CMBs may reflect more diffuse brain damage. A recent study using diffusion tensor imaging has shown that the presence of CMBs was associated with a poorer global microstructural integrity of the brain white matter, even when only a single CMB was present [33].

The location of CMBs is related to underlying vascular pathologies, such as hypertensive microangiopathy and CAA [8,9,10,11]. In our study, most of the subjects (87.5%) had CMBs in the deep or infratentorial regions, and only a few had CMBs in strictly lobar regions (12.5%). Thus, hypertensive microangiopathy, rather than CAA, might be the most likely underlying etiology of CMBs in our subjects. However, approximately 30% of subjects with deep CMBs also had CMBs in lobar regions. A histopathological study indicated the coexistence of both pathogenic mechanisms in patients having CMBs located in both deep and lobar regions [6]. Another recent study using in vivo imaging of amyloid with 11C-Pittsburgh compound B positron emission tomography in patients with cognitive impairment suggested that both the CAA and subcortical SVD contributed to the pathogenesis of lobar CMBs in patients having CMBs in both deep and lobar regions [34]. However, whether this is also the case in nondemented elderly subjects with SVD, as were our subjects, is currently unknown.

There are some limitations associated with our study. First, it had a cross-sectional design, and prospective studies are needed to establish causality. Second, we included subjects suspected to have SVD based on the presence of lacunar infarcts or WMLs, defined as at least Fazekas grade 1 on MRI. Although our subjects consisted of outpatients with a high prevalence of vascular risk factors, minimal WMLs (Fazekas grade 1) can be associated with non-SVD causes such as aging [35]. Unexpectedly, the mean age of the subjects with CMBs was significantly lower than that of the patients without. On the other hand, the prevalence of hypertension was significantly higher in subjects with CMBs than in those without. These findings might be associated with the recruitment of our subjects, in whom hypertension, rather than age, might have more strongly influenced the presence of CMBs. Third, although we found an association between cognition and lobar CMBs, we cannot rule out the influence of coexisting neuropathologies due to Alzheimer's disease. Fourth, the number of subjects in the present study was small, particularly when considering the subgroups with different CMB location. Further studies with larger sample sizes are required to confirm our findings. Finally, the FAB and MMSE, used for the assessment of cognitive functions in the present study, are screening tools, and more appropriate neuropsychological tests are needed to permit detailed analyses of the specific domains of cognitive function.

In conclusion, our study suggests that the presence of CMBs is associated with worse executive and global cognitive functions in nondemented elderly subjects with SVD on MRI. CMBs seemed to reflect hypertensive microangiopathy in this population, and those located in specific areas may play an important role in cognitive function.

We would like to thank Joe Matsuoka for his assistance with the statistical analysis. The present study was supported by the Juntendo Medical Research Institute.

The authors declare no financial or other conflict of interest.

1.
Pantoni L: Cerebral small vessel disease: from pathogenesis and clinical characteristics to therapeutic challenges. Lancet Neurol 2010;9:689-701.
2.
van der Flier WM, van Straaten EC, Barkhof F, Verdelho A, Madureira S, Pantoni L, Inzitari D, Erkinjuntti T, Crisby M, Waldemar G, Schmidt R, Fazekas F, Scheltens P: Small vessel disease and general cognitive function in nondisabled elderly: the LADIS study. Stroke 2005;36:2116-2120.
3.
Carey CL, Kramer JH, Josephson SA, Mungas D, Reed BR, Schuff N, Weiner MW, Chui HC: Subcortical lacunes are associated with executive dysfunction in cognitively normal elderly. Stroke 2008;39:397-402.
4.
Geerlings MI, Appelman AP, Vincken KL, Mali WP, van der Graaf Y: Association of white matter lesions and lacunar infarcts with executive functioning: the SMART-MR study. Am J Epidemiol 2009;170:1147-1155.
5.
Greenberg SM, Vernooij MW, Cordonnier C, Viswanathan A, Al-Shahi Salman R, Warach S, Launer LJ, Van Buchem MA, Breteler MM: Cerebral microbleeds: a guide to detection and interpretation. Lancet Neurol 2009;8:165-174.
6.
Fazekas F, Kleinert R, Roob G, Kleinert G, Kapeller P, Schmidt R, Hartung HP: Histopathologic analysis of foci of signal loss on gradient-echo T2*-weighted MR images in patients with spontaneous intracerebral hemorrhage: evidence of microangiopathy-related microbleeds. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1999;20:637-642.
7.
Shoamanesh A, Kwok CS, Benavente O: Cerebral microbleeds: histopathological correlation of neuroimaging. Cerebrovasc Dis 2011;32:528-534.
8.
Vernooij MW, van der Lugt A, Ikram MA, Wielopolski PA, Niessen WJ, Hofman A, Krestin GP, Breteler MM: Prevalence and risk factors of cerebral microbleeds: the Rotterdam Scan Study. Neurology 2008;70:1208-1214.
9.
Poels MM, Vernooij MW, Ikram MA, Hofman A, Krestin GP, van der Lugt A, Breteler MM: Prevalence and risk factors of cerebral microbleeds: an update of the Rotterdam Scan Study. Stroke 2010;41:S103-S106.
10.
Yamashiro K, Tanaka R, Okuma Y, Ueno Y, Tanaka Y, Hattori N, Urabe T: Associations of durations of antiplatelet use and vascular risk factors with the presence of cerebral microbleeds. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 2014;23:433-440.
11.
Schrag M, McAuley G, Pomakian J, Jiffry A, Tung S, Mueller C, Vinters HV, Haacke EM, Holshouser B, Kido D, Kirsch WM: Correlation of hypointensities in susceptibility-weighted images to tissue histology in dementia patients with cerebral amyloid angiopathy: a postmortem MRI study. Acta Neuropathol 2010;119:291-302.
12.
Charidimou A, Werring DJ: Cerebral microbleeds and cognition in cerebrovascular disease: an update. J Neurol Sci 2012;322:50-55.
13.
Qiu C, Cotch MF, Sigurdsson S, Jonsson PV, Jonsdottir MK, SveinbjFrnsdottir S, Eiriksdottir G, Klein R, Harris TB, van Buchem MA, Gudnason V, Launer LJ: Cerebral microbleeds, retinopathy, and dementia: the AGES-Reykjavik study. Neurology 2010;75:2221-2228.
14.
Yakushiji Y, Nishiyama M, Yakushiji S, Hirotsu T, Uchino A, Nakajima J, Eriguchi M, Nanri Y, Hara M, Horikawa E, Kuroda Y: Brain microbleeds and global cognitive function in adults without neurological disorder. Stroke 2008;39:3323-3328.
15.
Takashima Y, Mori T, Hashimoto M, Kinukawa N, Uchino A, Yuzuriha T, Yao H: Clinical correlating factors and cognitive function in community-dwelling healthy subjects with cerebral microbleeds. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 2011;20:105-110.
16.
Poels MM, Ikram MA, van der Lugt A, Westendorp RG, Bollen EL, Blauw GJ, Greenberg SM, van Buchem MA: Cerebral microbleeds are associated with worse cognitive function: the Rotterdam Scan study. Neurology 2012;78:326-333.
17.
van Es AC, van der Grond J, de Craen AJ, Westendorp RG, Bollen EL, Blauw GJ, Greenberg SM, van Buchem MA: Cerebral microbleeds and cognitive functioning in the PROSPER study. Neurology 2011;77:1446-1452.
18.
Werring DJ, Frazer DW, Coward LJ, Westendorp RG, Bollen EL, Blauw GJ, Greenberg SM, van Buchem MA: Cognitive dysfunction in patients with cerebral microbleeds on T2*-weighted gradient-echo MRI. Brain 2004;127:2265-2275.
19.
Gregoire SM, Scheffler G, Jager HR, Yousry TA, Brown MM, Kallis C, Cipolotti L, Werring DJ: Strictly lobar microbleeds are associated with executive impairment in patients with ischemic stroke or transient ischemic attack. Stroke 2013;44:1267-1272.
20.
Patel B, Lawrence AJ, Chung AW, Rich P, Mackinnon AD, Morris RG, Barrick TR, Markus HS: Cerebral microbleeds and cognition in patients with symptomatic small vessel disease. Stroke 2013;44:356-361.
21.
Zhang M, Chen M, Wang Q, Yun W, Zhang Z, Yin Q, Huang Q, Zhu W: Relationship between cerebral microbleeds and cognitive function in lacunar infarct. J Int Med Res 2013;41:347-355.
22.
Yates PA, Villemagne VL, Ellis KA, Desmond PM, Masters CL, Rowe CC: Cerebral microbleeds: a review of clinical, genetic, and neuroimaging associations. Front Neurol 2014;4:205.
23.
Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA: MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer's dementia and normal aging. AJR Am J Roentgenol 1987;149:351-356.
24.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), ed 4. Washington, American Psychiatric Association, 1994.
25.
Gregoire SM, Chaudhary UJ, Brown MM, Yousry TA, Kallis C, Jager HR, Werring DJ: The Microbleed Anatomical Rating Scale (MARS): reliability of a tool to map brain microbleeds. Neurology 2009;73:1759-1766.
26.
Koga H, Yuzuriha T, Yao H, Endo K, Hiejima S, Takashima Y, Sadanaga F, Matsumoto T, Uchino A, Ogomori K, Ichimiya A, Uchimura H, Tashiro N: Quantitative MRI findings and cognitive impairment among community dwelling elderly subjects. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002;72:737-741.
27.
Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B: The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology 2000;55:1621-1626.
28.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: ‘Mini-mental state'. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198.
29.
O'Sullivan M, Morris RG, Markus HS: Brief cognitive assessment for patients with cerebral small vessel disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:1140-1145.
30.
Hoshi T, Yamagami H, Furukado S, Miwa K, Tanaka M, Sakaguchi M, Sakoda S, Kitagawa K: Serum inflammatory proteins and frontal lobe dysfunction in patients with cardiovascular risk factors. Eur J Neurol 2010;17:1134-1140.
31.
van Norden AG, van den Berg HA, de Laat KF, Gons RA, van Dijk EJ, de Leeuw FE: Frontal and temporal microbleeds are related to cognitive function: the Radboud University Nijmegen Diffusion Tensor and Magnetic Resonance Cohort (RUN DMC) study. Stroke 2011;42:3382-3386.
32.
Tekin S, Cummings JL: Frontal-subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: an update. J Psychosom Res 2002;53:647-654.
33.
Akoudad S, de Groot M, Koudstaal PJ, van der Lugt A, Niessen WJ, Hofman A, Ikram MA, Vernooij MW: Cerebral microbleeds are related to loss of white matter structural integrity. Neurology 2013;81:1930-1937.
34.
Park JH, Seo SW, Kim C, Kim GH, Noh HJ, Kim ST, Kwak KC, Yoon U, Lee JM, Lee JW, Shin JS, Kim CH, Noh Y, Cho H, Kim HJ, Yoon CW, Oh SJ, Kim JS, Choe YS, Lee KH, Lee JH, Ewers M, Weiner MW, Werring DJ, Na DL: Pathogenesis of cerebral microbleeds: in vivo imaging of amyloid and subcortical ischemic small vessel disease in 226 individuals with cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 2013;73:584-593.
35.
Gouw AA, Seewann A, van der Flier WM, Barkhof F, Rozemuller AM, Scheltens P, Geurts JJ: Heterogeneity of small vessel disease: a systematic review of MRI and histopathology correlations. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82:126-135.
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
Open Access License: This is an Open Access article licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 3.0 Unported license (CC BY-NC) (www.karger.com/OA-license), applicable to the online version of the article only. Distribution permitted for non-commercial purposes only.
Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug.
Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.