We quantified changes in the auditory acuity of 675 aging adults (mean age 71.1 years, 52.0% female, mean follow-up 4.4 years ± 0.2) of an ongoing cohort study with a pure-tone audiogram and a speech-in-noise test. Generalized estimating equation models were used to study the association between hearing loss and the progression with age, sex, education, cognition, BMI, blood pressure, having type 2 diabetes mellitus, cholesterol ratio, smoking and alcohol consumption. The mean progression of hearing loss was 0.29 and 1.35 dB/year (low and high frequencies). Progression of hearing loss was associated with baseline hearing thresholds. Besides, the presence of type 2 diabetes, smoking, age, sex and time were associated with worse hearing at baseline, but there was no statistical evidence that the tested determinants were associated with progression of hearing loss. This finding indicates that the 4-year progression of hearing loss in older adults in this study is not influenced by the measured determinants. More research with multiple follow-up rounds is desired.

Age-related hearing loss affects over a billion people worldwide [GBD, 2016], and its prevalence keeps rising due to aging of the population [Olusanya et al., 2014]. As the fourth leading cause of years lived with disability in developed countries [GBD, 2016], hearing loss has a major impact on daily life and is associated with high health care costs.

The nature of hearing loss in older adults is progressive [Cruickshanks et al., 2010]. From approximately the fifth decade onwards, hearing thresholds and speech understanding in noise gradually decline. Multiple determinants are thought to influence the onset and severity of hearing loss in older adults, namely demographic factors such as age, sex and social economic status [Agrawal et al., 2008], medical factors such as cardiovascular disease, cognition, diabetes mellitus, cholesterol level and obesity [Gopinath et al., 2010; Helzner et al., 2011; Akinpelu et al., 2014; Taljaard et al., 2016; Dhanda and Taheri, 2017], lifestyle-related factors such as noise exposure, smoking and an inverse correlation for alcohol consumption [Agrawal et al., 2008; Dawes et al., 2014; Rigters et al., 2016; Lin et al., 2017] and genetic susceptibility [Hoffmann et al., 2016]. The rate of the progression of hearing loss varies widely among people of the same age [Gates and Mills, 2005]. The decline of pure-tone thresholds over time has been found associated with several factors such as age, being male or female, blood pressure, obesity, having diabetes, cognitive impairment and manual occupation [Brant et al., 1996; Mitchell et al., 2009; Kiely et al., 2012; Linssen et al., 2014]. When reviewed critically, some studies that claim to address associations on progression, in fact addressed the incidence of hearing loss [Cruickshanks et al., 2015]. In general, previous studies lack the combination of (1) a representative aging population with a wide range of hearing (instead of a group of hearing-impaired compared to a group of normal-hearing participants), where (2) auditory acuity (thresholds and speech perception) as well as the possible determinants were measured and (3) sufficient statistical methods were used.

Knowing which patients are at risk of more rapid deterioration of hearing acuity could influence counseling, rehabilitation and possible treatment of the underlying condition. The purpose of this study is to identify if and to what extent the progression of hearing loss in older adults over time is associated with potentially relevant determinants.

Study Population

This study is embedded in the Rotterdam Study, an ongoing population-based prospective cohort study designed to investigate the health of aging people [Hofman et al., 2015]. The population consists of inhabitants aged 55 years and above of the Ommoord district in the city of Rotterdam, the Netherlands. In 2011, hearing assessment was implemented in the study protocol, and participants are invited for reassessment approximately every 4 years. In 2015 the first group of participants was invited for their second hearing assessment. For the current study, we included participants with two hearing assessments (n = 722 from 5,762). Those who had been reassessed within less than 3 years (n = 18) and those with conductive hearing loss on the best hearing ear (n = 29) were excluded. This resulted in a total number of 675 participants. Included participants did not significantly differ in age, sex and mean hearing loss from participants with one hearing assessment.

The Rotterdam Study has been approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Erasmus University Medical Center and by the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport of the Netherlands, implementing the Population Screening Act: Rotterdam Study. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the study and to have their information obtained from treating physicians.

Hearing Assessment

Participants were tested by a single qualified professional in a soundproof booth at the research center. TDH-39 headphones and a clinical audiometer (Decos audiology workstation, version 210.2.6 with AudioNigma interface) were used. A pure-tone audiogram and speech-in-noise test were performed. First, pure-tone thresholds were measured according to the ISO-standard 8253-1 [ISO, 2010]. Air conduction (0.25, 0.50, 1, 2, 4 and 8 kHz) and bone conduction (0.5 and 4 kHz, due to limited time) were measured for both ears. Masking was done according to the method of Hood [1960]. Bone conduction thresholds at 4 kHz were +10 dB adjusted afterwards [Margolis et al., 2013]. The best hearing ear was determined by calculating the mean threshold over all frequencies. If hearing was equal between both ears, alternately the right or left was chosen. On the best hearing ear, we calculated low- (mean of 0.25, 0.50 and 1 kHz), speech- (mean of 0.50, 1, 2 and 4 kHz), and high- (mean of 2, 4 and 8 kHz) frequency hearing thresholds to determine the low-, speech-, and high-frequency hearing loss. To eliminate clinically relevant conductive hearing loss, test results of participants with an air-bone gap of 15 dB or more were excluded from the analyses.

Subsequently, a simplified speech-in-noise test was done to quantify the speech recognition ability in noise. We performed the digits-in-noise (DIN) test on the best-hearing ear [Koole et al., 2016]. Participants repeated digit triplets in an automated adaptive procedure, while the signal-to-noise ratio was changed according to the correctness of the answer. This resulted in a speech reception threshold which represents a speech-in-noise ratio for 50% correctly repeated triplets. A higher threshold means a worse ability of understanding speech in noise. After a preliminary evaluation of our hearing data, suprathreshold noise levels were changed from 55 dB at baseline to 65 dB during follow-up. To avoid confounding with the peripheral hearing level, we additionally adjusted for the high-frequency hearing thresholds in the subsequent analysis concerning the DIN test.

Progression of hearing loss was defined as devaluation of hearing thresholds from baseline to reassessment.

Determinants

Information on the potentially relevant determinants was acquired through a home interview, physical examination and blood sampling at baseline. Educational level was classified as having completed primary, secondary or higher schooling. Cognition was defined as the score on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Body mass index (BMI) was calculated through weight and length. Systolic blood pressure was measured twice on the right brachial artery with the participant in a sitting position and in between a resting period of 5 min. The mean of the two values was used. Cases of type 2 diabetes were identified from general practitioners’ records. If we could not retrieve this information, diabetes mellitus was considered present if the glucose measurement was abnormal or if the participant used antidiabetic drugs. Abnormal glucose measurement was defined as fasting glucose 7 mmol/L or more, or (if unavailable) as nonfasting glucose 11 mmol/L or more. Cholesterol ratio was calculated as the quotient of serum total cholesterol and high-density cholesterol. Smoking was classified as never, former or current. Alcohol consumption was categorized as nondrinker, light consumer (1 unit of alcohol per day for women and 1–2 units of alcohol per day for men) or above average consumer (more than 1 unit of alcohol per day for women and more than 2 units of alcohol per day for men) [Dawson and Room, 2000].

Statistics

To examine the characteristics of the study population, we calculated mean and standard deviation or percentage for all demographics. In the first analysis we calculated the association between baseline thresholds and the progression of hearing loss using a linear regression model, accounting for the different participant demographics. In a second analysis we used generalized estimating equations, to assess the effect of the different determinants on the progression of hearing loss [Zeger et al., 1988]. Population-average progression was defined as the main effect of time. To study the differences in progression of hearing loss between subgroups of the population, we allowed for interactions between time and all the determinants. Separate generalized estimating equation models were used for each of the hearing outcomes: the low-, speech-, and high-frequency hearing thresholds and the outcome of the DIN test; the speech reception threshold (SRT). To account for the correlation between measurements coming from the same subject, we assumed an exchangeable correlation structure. We started by specifying an elaborate model including the main effects of all the determinants as well as higher-order terms such as interactions of baseline determinants with time and a quadratic effect of age. The higher-order terms were then tested using multivariate Wald tests. A p value of 0.05 or less was considered significant. All higher-order terms were not found to be significantly different from zero and thus were dropped from the final models. The models including the higher-order terms are shown in Table 1 of the supplementary material (see www.karger.com/doi/10.1159/000492203 for all online suppl. material). Missing data were assumed to be missing completely at random. Analyses were performed using R 3.4.1. [R Core Team, 2017] and package geepack 1.2-1 [Yan, 2002; Yan and Fine, 2004; Hølsgaard et al., 2006].

Characteristics of the Study Population

A total of 650 complete cases were analyzed. The mean (SD, range) age at baseline was 71.1 (4.1, 66–87) years, and 52% were females. The mean (range) follow-up was 4.4 (3.3–5.1) years. All relevant characteristics of the study population are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of the study population at baseline (n = 650)

Demographic characteristics of the study population at baseline (n = 650)
Demographic characteristics of the study population at baseline (n = 650)

Progression of Hearing Loss

The difference in hearing thresholds was large (95% confidence interval at baseline for the mean threshold of lower frequencies 6.7 with 36.7 dB, of higher frequencies 18.3 with 68.3 dB). Pure-tone thresholds at baseline were worse for higher than for lower frequencies, and thresholds worsened with each age category (Fig. 1). The prevalence of age-related hearing impairment according to the WHO (pure-tone audiogram 0.5, 1, 2 and 4 kHz > 25 dB loss at best-hearing ear) at baseline was 48.5%. The prevalence rose with each age category (respectively 38.8% for subjects aged 66–69 years, 53.1% for 70–79 years and 81.8% for 80–87 years). At the follow-up, this was 61.4% (53.6, 65.5 and 84.8%).

Fig. 1.

Average pure-tone thresholds of the study population at baseline. Thresholds shown for three age categories. HL, hearing level.

Fig. 1.

Average pure-tone thresholds of the study population at baseline. Thresholds shown for three age categories. HL, hearing level.

Close modal

Figure 2 shows the mean threshold at baseline in relation to the mean threshold at follow-up, again displayed for the three age categories. All participants left and above of the drawn line showed progression of hearing loss. An increase in hearing thresholds – progressive hearing loss – was present for 512 participants (78.8%). The average decline of hearing loss was 0.29 dB/year in the low frequencies and 1.35 dB/year in the high frequencies. The progression was significantly associated with the baseline thresholds. For the lower frequencies an effect estimate of –0.07 dB per 4 years of follow-up (p value 0.010) was found and for the higher frequencies an effect estimate of –0.06 dB per 4 years of follow-up (p value 0.002), after correcting for age, sex and the other determinants. In other words, for approximately every 16 dB elevation of the baseline threshold, 1 dB less progression in the follow-up period was expected.

Fig. 2.

Average hearing threshold at baseline and follow-up. Average pure-tone threshold for 0.5, 1, 2 and 4 kHz at baseline in relation to the average threshold at follow-up shown for the three age categories. Every participant left of the line suffers decline of hearing during follow-up. HL, hearing level.

Fig. 2.

Average hearing threshold at baseline and follow-up. Average pure-tone threshold for 0.5, 1, 2 and 4 kHz at baseline in relation to the average threshold at follow-up shown for the three age categories. Every participant left of the line suffers decline of hearing during follow-up. HL, hearing level.

Close modal

Determinants

Using the full model specification, none of the interaction terms with time was found statistically significant at the 0.05 significance level. That is, there was no statistical evidence to support a difference in progression of hearing loss by the determinants investigated in this study. The interaction terms were therefore dropped from the final generalized estimating equation models. Table 2 displays the results of the final models for the low-, speech-, and high-frequency thresholds, as well as for the SRT. The initial, full models can be found in the online supplementary material.

Table 2.

Effect estimates from the final generalized estimating equation models for hearing acuity

Effect estimates from the final generalized estimating equation models for hearing acuity
Effect estimates from the final generalized estimating equation models for hearing acuity

Worse hearing thresholds in the low frequencies were associated with time, aging, being of female sex and being a current smoker. Increase in BMI and having type 2 diabetes were border significant. Worse hearing thresholds in the speech and high frequencies were associated with time, aging, being of male sex, having type 2 diabetes mellitus and being a current smoker.

Worse hearing on the SRT was inversely associated with time. Thus, speech reception seemed to improve over time. Furthermore, the SRT was associated with age, being of female sex and having type 2 diabetes.

The implications of our outcomes are visualized in Figure 3 by means of progression lines for high-frequency hearing loss over time for different groups of participants. Smokers with diabetes mellitus had initial higher pure-tone losses at baseline (both males and females) but the progression of hearing loss in both groups was equal. The progression of hearing loss was also equal for males and females.

Fig. 3.

Progression of high-frequency hearing loss over time for different categories of participants. HL, hearing level. All progression lines and 95% confidence intervals shown are for a participant with median outcomes (70 years, secondary educational level, MMSE score 28, BMI 27, systolic blood pressure 149 mm Hg, average drinker, cholesterol ratio 3.63). Only sex, presence of diabetes, and smoking status differed.

Fig. 3.

Progression of high-frequency hearing loss over time for different categories of participants. HL, hearing level. All progression lines and 95% confidence intervals shown are for a participant with median outcomes (70 years, secondary educational level, MMSE score 28, BMI 27, systolic blood pressure 149 mm Hg, average drinker, cholesterol ratio 3.63). Only sex, presence of diabetes, and smoking status differed.

Close modal

This study showed that the progression of hearing loss over a short time was not affected by age, sex, educational level, cognition, BMI, systolic blood pressure, presence of type 2 diabetes mellitus, cholesterol ratio, smoking and alcohol consumption. On the other hand, higher initial hearing thresholds had a decelerating effect on the progression rate. A baseline difference of approximately 16 dB resulted in 1 dB less progression over the 4 years of follow-up, which effect is substantial in view of the average 1.35-dB decline of hearing loss per year.

Previous studies have reported inconsistent effects of the initial hearing level on the rate of progression. In line with our results, poorer baseline thresholds for the higher frequencies (4–8 kHz) were found to be associated with less progression in hearing loss [Gates and Cooper, 1991]. In that same study no effect of lower frequencies (0.25–1 kHz) was found. A similar result was also found in a study in which the progression of hearing loss was defined as a deterioration of > 5 dB on 0.5–4 kHz [Cruickshanks et al., 2003]. In contrast, worse hearing thresholds were associated with a faster decline in another large cohort-based study [Linssen et al., 2014]. The follow-up period in that study was longer (12 years), which could explain different findings. However, the authors presented only the results of the univariate analysis between progression and baseline thresholds, while we presented ours after correcting for age and sex. Because their study population included far younger participants than ours (24.0–83.7 years), the found effect might also have been a consequence of a different etiology of hearing loss, for example a genetic cause.

In our second analysis, the generalized estimating equation models, none of the determinants had an effect on the progression of hearing loss according to the p value chosen to test statistical significance, other than the elapse of time itself. Remarkably, the time effect found on the SRT was inverse. Time seemed to have an inhibitory effect on speech perception loss. This can be ascribed to having adjusted the DIN test after a first cross-sectional evaluation of our hearing data [Koole et al., 2016]. To reduce the confounding effect with pure-tone thresholds, suprathreshold noise levels were adjusted from 55 dB at baseline to 65 dB during follow-up, to reduce the confounding effect with pure-tone thresholds.

Age had no significant effect on the progression of hearing loss. This is surprising, as the ISO standard [ISO, 2010] uses a model with a consistently increasing progression of hearing loss with age, which is in line with several studies that show accelerated progression with higher age [Kiely et al., 2012; Linssen et al., 2014]. These studies were able to use linear mixed models because they had multiple audiometric measurements. Because in the present study only two audiometric measurements per participant were available, our analysis was restricted to generalized estimating equations rather than mixed-effect models. Still, the time span between the two measurements may have been too short to identify a significant difference in progression. Furthermore, due to interpretation purposes and the endogenous nature of some of the exposures only the baseline values of the determinants were used as exposure variables.

Another difference with the studies of Kiely et al. and Linssen et al. is that our study population was older and thus more prone to a higher prevalence of age-related hearing loss. In older adults, not only the effect of aging itself, but also a ceiling effect has been described: the more the loss of high-frequency hearing, the less the rate of progression, possibly because a maximum loss was being reached [Brant and Fozard, 1990; Wattamwar et al., 2017]. Therefore, we found more progression in the lower than in the higher frequencies. With an average age of 90 years, the study population of Wattamwar et al. was much older than ours, but it may well be that this stagnation of progression is already apparent at an earlier age, which may counteract a possible accelerated progression of hearing loss at higher ages as suggested by the ISO standard.

Like age, also sex was not associated with the progression of hearing loss. This is in line with the adaptation of the new ISO standard [ISO, 2017], in which sex differences are much smaller than in the older version [ISO, 2010]. We did find that sex was associated with the onset of hearing loss. Worse hearing thresholds in the lower frequencies were associated with being female and worse hearing in the speech and higher frequencies was associated with being male. Former cross-sectional studies found that women have better high-frequency hearing and that men have better low-frequency hearing [Rigters et al., 2016]. This is possibly explained by the assumption that men are at higher risk of noise-induced hearing loss.

Because age-related hearing loss is thought to be a risk factor for the onset of dementia [Livingston et al., 2017], we studied the risk of developing dementia using poor performance on the DIN test. Prior studies mainly focused on peripheral hearing loss (pure-tone audiometry), while we hoped with the DIN to reflect the higher auditory function. Dementia was stated as a MMSE score of 26 or lower, and we calculated the odds ratio of the onset of dementia according to a worse performance on the DIN test using a univariable logistic regression model, due to the fact that only 40 of the 559 nondemented subjects at baseline developed dementia (7.2%). We found an odds ratio for dementia according to worse performance on the DIN test of 1.05 (CI 0.99–1.12) with a p value of 0.065; however, this result should be looked at with care, since other baseline characteristics were not taken into account for this analysis.

Some studies identified determinants that had an effect on the progression of hearing loss, such as a lower cognitive impairment, hypertension, having had a manual occupation and waist circumference [Kiely et al., 2012, Linssen et al., 2014]. Still, all effects were very small and may only become apparent after a longer time.

Although in the present study type 2 diabetes and smoking were not associated with the progression of hearing loss, they were associated with the onset of age-related hearing loss. We found a significant effect regardless of the correction for cardiovascular confounders. This association of type 2 diabetes [Akinpelu et al., 2014] and smoking [Dawes et al., 2014; Chang et al., 2016; Rigters et al., 2016] with the prevalence of hearing loss in older adults was shown before. Possible hypotheses on the underlying pathogenesis of diabetes or smoking and hearing loss could be microangiopathy of the stria vascularis [Fukushima et al., 2006], neuropathy or mitochondrial damage [Helzner and Contrera, 2016]. For diabetes we found no significant effect in the lower frequencies and a larger effect in the higher frequencies when compared to the speech frequencies. This could indicate a vascular cause taking into account that the base of the cochlea is more vascularized than the apex and thus has a greater blood supply. The higher frequencies may therefore be more affected by microangiopathy [Shi, 2011].

One of the limitations of this study is that with two measurements per subject the statistical modeling options for studying the progression of hearing loss were limited (e.g. mixed-effects models with random slopes are not feasible). We therefore used generalized estimation equation models to fit the data best [Zeger et al., 1988]. With more follow-up rounds planned, we expect to report on this in the future. Another limitation was the short follow-up time. We consider our results therefore to reflect the short-term effects of hearing loss. Also, we did not have any information on noise exposure. Although in previous research no relation between noise-induced hearing loss and the progression of hearing loss was found [Lee et al., 2005], some cross-sectional studies did find associations with the prevalence of hearing impairment. We tried to account for this by including educational level in our analysis.

The strength of this study lies in the representativeness of our cohort as compared to the general population. At baseline there was a natural distribution of age and of all levels of hearing loss. Many earlier studies compared hearing-impaired and normal-hearing participants, while we studied the whole spectrum of hearing loss. Second, we structurally collected data and called in the help of a statistician for the analyses. Last, the prevalence of age-related hearing impairment was in line with prevalence numbers of other cohort studies using the WHO criteria [Agrawal et al., 2008; Lin et al., 2011; Mitchell et al., 2011].

In summary, in this study we showed that although hearing loss in the aging population was associated with type 2 diabetes, smoking, age and sex, we did not find an association with the progression of hearing loss for any of these determinants. Worse baseline hearing levels were associated with a less rapid progression, which could be proof of a ceiling effect in hearing deterioration.

This study indicates that the 4-year progression of hearing loss among older adults aged 66–87 years is not influenced by the measured demographic and clinical determinants. To further clarify this, more research with multiple follow-up rounds is warranted.

We would like to thank Tekla Enser for conducting all audiometry at our research center. This work was supported by a grant from the Heinsius Houbolt foundation and a grant from Cochlear.

There is no conflict of interest.

1.
Agrawal
Y
,
Platz
EA
,
Niparko
JK
.
Prevalence of hearing loss and differences by demographic characteristics among US adults: data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1999-2004
.
Arch Intern Med
.
2008
Jul
;
168
(
14
):
1522
30
.
[PubMed]
0003-9926
2.
Akinpelu
OV
,
Mujica-Mota
M
,
Daniel
SJ
.
Is type 2 diabetes mellitus associated with alterations in hearing? A systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Laryngoscope
.
2014
Mar
;
124
(
3
):
767
76
.
[PubMed]
0023-852X
3.
Brant
LJ
,
Fozard
JL
.
Age changes in pure-tone hearing thresholds in a longitudinal study of normal human aging
.
J Acoust Soc Am
.
1990
Aug
;
88
(
2
):
813
20
.
[PubMed]
0001-4966
4.
Brant
LJ
,
Gordon-Salant
S
,
Pearson
JD
,
Klein
LL
,
Morrell
CH
,
Metter
EJ
, et al
Risk factors related to age-associated hearing loss in the speech frequencies
.
J Am Acad Audiol
.
1996
Jun
;
7
(
3
):
152
60
.
[PubMed]
1050-0545
5.
Chang
J
,
Ryou
N
,
Jun
HJ
,
Hwang
SY
,
Song
JJ
,
Chae
SW
.
Effect of cigarette smoking and passive smoking on hearing impairment: data from a population–based study
.
PLoS One
.
2016
Jan
;
11
(
1
):
e0146608
.
[PubMed]
1932-6203
6.
Cruickshanks
KJ
,
Nondahl
DM
,
Dalton
DS
,
Fischer
ME
,
Klein
BE
,
Klein
R
, et al
Smoking, central adiposity, and poor glycemic control increase risk of hearing impairment
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2015
May
;
63
(
5
):
918
24
.
[PubMed]
0002-8614
7.
Cruickshanks
KJ
,
Nondahl
DM
,
Tweed
TS
,
Wiley
TL
,
Klein
BE
,
Klein
R
, et al
Education, occupation, noise exposure history and the 10-yr cumulative incidence of hearing impairment in older adults
.
Hear Res
.
2010
Jun
;
264
(
1-2
):
3
9
.
[PubMed]
0378-5955
8.
Cruickshanks KJ, Tweed TS, Wiley TL, Klein BE, Klein R, Chappell R, et al. The 5-year incidence and progression of hearing loss: the epidemiology of hearing loss study. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2003 Oct;129(10):1041–6.
9.
Dawes
P
,
Cruickshanks
KJ
,
Moore
DR
,
Edmondson-Jones
M
,
McCormack
A
,
Fortnum
H
, et al
Cigarette smoking, passive smoking, alcohol consumption, and hearing loss
.
J Assoc Res Otolaryngol
.
2014
Aug
;
15
(
4
):
663
74
.
[PubMed]
1525-3961
10.
Dawson
DA
,
Room
R
.
Towards agreement on ways to measure and report drinking patterns and alcohol-related problems in adult general population surveys: the Skarpö conference overview
.
J Subst Abuse
.
2000
;
12
(
1-2
):
1
21
.
[PubMed]
0899-3289
11.
Dhanda
N
,
Taheri
S
.
A narrative review of obesity and hearing loss
.
Int J Obes
.
2017
Jul
;
41
(
7
):
1066
73
.
[PubMed]
0307-0565
12.
Fukushima
H
,
Cureoglu
S
,
Schachern
PA
,
Paparella
MM
,
Harada
T
,
Oktay
MF
.
Effects of type 2 diabetes mellitus on cochlear structure in humans
.
Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg
.
2006
Sep
;
132
(
9
):
934
8
.
[PubMed]
0886-4470
13.
Gates
GA
,
Cooper
JC
.
Incidence of hearing decline in the elderly
.
Acta Otolaryngol
.
1991
;
111
(
2
):
240
8
.
[PubMed]
0001-6489
14.
Gates
GA
,
Mills
JH
.
Presbycusis
.
Lancet
.
2005
Sep
;
366
(
9491
):
1111
20
.
[PubMed]
0140-6736
15.
GBD 2015 Disease and Injury Incidence and Prevalence Collaborators: Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 310 diseases and injuries, 1990-2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016
.
Lancet
.
2016
;
388
:
1545
602
.
[PubMed]
0140-6736
16.
Gopinath
B
,
Flood
VM
,
McMahon
CM
,
Burlutsky
G
,
Brand-Miller
J
,
Mitchell
P
.
Dietary glycemic load is a predictor of age-related hearing loss in older adults
.
J Nutr
.
2010
Dec
;
140
(
12
):
2207
12
.
[PubMed]
0022-3166
17.
Helzner
EP
,
Contrera
KJ
.
Type 2 diabetes and hearing impairment
.
Curr Diab Rep
.
2016
Jan
;
16
(
1
):
3
.
[PubMed]
1534-4827
18.
Helzner
EP
,
Patel
AS
,
Pratt
S
,
Sutton-Tyrrell
K
,
Cauley
JA
,
Talbott
E
, et al
Hearing sensitivity in older adults: associations with cardiovascular risk factors in the health, aging and body composition study
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2011
Jun
;
59
(
6
):
972
9
.
[PubMed]
0002-8614
19.
Hoffmann
TJ
,
Keats
BJ
,
Yoshikawa
N
,
Schaefer
C
,
Risch
N
,
Lustig
LR
: A large genome-wide association study of age-related hearing impairment using electronic health records. PLoS Genet
2016
;12: eCollection 2016. DOI:
20.
Hofman
A
,
Brusselle
GG
,
Darwish Murad
S
,
van Duijn
CM
,
Franco
OH
,
Goedegebure
A
, et al
The Rotterdam Study: 2016 objectives and design update
.
Eur J Epidemiol
.
2015
Aug
;
30
(
8
):
661
708
.
[PubMed]
0393-2990
21.
Hølsgaard
S
,
Halekoh
U
,
Yan
J
.
The R package geepack for generalized estimating equations
.
J Stat Softw
.
2005
;
15
(
2
):
1
11
. 1548-7660
22.
Hood
JD
.
The principles and practice of bone conduction audiometry: A review of the present position
.
Laryngoscope
.
1960
Sep
;
70
(
9
):
1211
28
.
[PubMed]
0023-852X
23.
ISO
, International Organization for Standardization: Acoustics-Audiometric test methods-Part 1: Basic pure tone air and bone conduction threshold audiometry. Geneva
2010
; ISO 8253-1.
24.
ISO
, International Organization for Standardization: Acoustics-Reference zero for the calibration of audiometric equipment-Part 1: Reference equivalent threshold sound pressure levels for pure tones and supra-aural earphones. Geneva
2017
; ISO 389-1.
25.
Kiely
KM
,
Gopinath
B
,
Mitchell
P
,
Luszcz
M
,
Anstey
KJ
.
Cognitive, health, and sociodemographic predictors of longitudinal decline in hearing acuity among older adults
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
.
2012
Sep
;
67
(
9
):
997
1003
.
[PubMed]
1079-5006
26.
Koole
A
,
Nagtegaal
AP
,
Homans
NC
,
Hofman
A
,
Baatenburg de Jong
RJ
,
Goedegebure
A
.
Using the digits-in-noise test to estimate age-related hearing loss
.
Ear Hear
.
2016
Sep-Oct
;
37
(
5
):
508
13
.
[PubMed]
0196-0202
27.
Lee
FS
,
Matthews
LJ
,
Dubno
JR
,
Mills
JH
.
Longitudinal study of pure-tone thresholds in older persons
.
Ear Hear
.
2005
Feb
;
26
(
1
):
1
11
.
[PubMed]
0196-0202
28.
Lin
FR
,
Thorpe
R
,
Gordon-Salant
S
,
Ferrucci
L
.
Hearing loss prevalence and risk factors among older adults in the United States
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
.
2011
May
;
66
(
5
):
582
90
.
[PubMed]
1079-5006
29.
Lin
YY
,
Chen
HC
,
Lai
WS
,
Wu
LW
,
Wang
CH
,
Lee
JC
, et al
Gender differences in the association between moderate alcohol consumption and hearing threshold shifts
.
Sci Rep
.
2017
May
;
7
(
1
):
2201
.
[PubMed]
2045-2322
30.
Linssen
AM
,
van Boxtel
MP
,
Joore
MA
,
Anteunis
LJ
.
Predictors of hearing acuity: cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis
.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
.
2014
Jun
;
69
(
6
):
759
65
.
[PubMed]
1079-5006
31.
Livingston
G
,
Sommerlad
A
,
Orgeta
V
,
Costafreda
SG
,
Huntley
J
,
Ames
D
, et al
Dementia prevention, intervention, and care
.
Lancet
.
2017
Dec
;
390
(
10113
):
2673
734
.
[PubMed]
0140-6736
32.
Margolis
RH
,
Eikelboom
RH
,
Johnson
C
,
Ginter
SM
,
Swanepoel
W
,
Moore
BC
.
False air-bone gaps at 4 kHz in listeners with normal hearing and sensorineural hearing loss
.
Int J Audiol
.
2013
Aug
;
52
(
8
):
526
32
.
[PubMed]
1499-2027
33.
Mitchell
P
,
Gopinath
B
,
McMahon
CM
,
Rochtchina
E
,
Wang
JJ
,
Boyages
SC
, et al
Relationship of Type 2 diabetes to the prevalence, incidence and progression of age-related hearing loss
.
Diabet Med
.
2009
May
;
26
(
5
):
483
8
.
[PubMed]
0742-3071
34.
Mitchell
P
,
Gopinath
B
,
Wang
JJ
,
McMahon
CM
,
Schneider
J
,
Rochtchina
E
, et al
Five-year incidence and progression of hearing impairment in an older population
.
Ear Hear
.
2011
Mar-Apr
;
32
(
2
):
251
7
.
[PubMed]
0196-0202
35.
Olusanya
BO
,
Neumann
KJ
,
Saunders
JE
.
The global burden of disabling hearing impairment: a call to action
.
Bull World Health Organ
.
2014
May
;
92
(
5
):
367
73
.
[PubMed]
0042-9686
36.
R Core Team
. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria,
2017
. URL https://www.R-project.org/
37.
Rigters
SC
,
Metselaar
M
,
Wieringa
MH
,
Baatenburg de Jong
RJ
,
Hofman
A
,
Goedegebure
A
.
Contributing determinants to hearing loss in elderly men and women: results from the population-based Rotterdam Study
.
Audiol Neurotol
.
2016
;
21
Suppl 1
:
10
5
.
[PubMed]
1420-3030
38.
Shi
X
.
Physiopathology of the cochlear microcirculation
.
Hear Res
.
2011
Dec
;
282
(
1-2
):
10
24
.
[PubMed]
0378-5955
39.
Taljaard
DS
,
Olaithe
M
,
Brennan-Jones
CG
,
Eikelboom
RH
,
Bucks
RS
.
The relationship between hearing impairment and cognitive function: a meta-analysis in adults
.
Clin Otolaryngol
.
2016
Dec
;
41
(
6
):
718
29
.
[PubMed]
1749-4478
40.
Wattamwar
K
,
Qian
ZJ
,
Otter
J
,
Leskowitz
MJ
,
Caruana
FF
,
Siedlecki
B
, et al
Increases in the rate of age-related hearing loss in the older old
.
JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg
.
2017
Jan
;
143
(
1
):
41
5
.
[PubMed]
2168-6181
41.
Yan
J
,
Fine
J
.
Estimating equations for association structures
.
Stat Med
.
2004
Mar
;
23
(
6
):
859
74
.
[PubMed]
0277-6715
42.
Yan
J
.
Geepack: yet another package for generalized estimating equations
.
Equations R-News
.
2002
;
2/3
:
12
4
.
43.
Zeger
SL
,
Liang
KY
,
Albert
PS
.
Models for longitudinal data: a generalized estimating equation approach
.
Biometrics
.
1988
Dec
;
44
(
4
):
1049
60
.
[PubMed]
0006-341X
Open Access License / Drug Dosage / Disclaimer
This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes as well as any distribution of modified material requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.